Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

Only $11.99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art
Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art
Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art
Ebook440 pages5 hours

Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars

()

Read preview

About this ebook

This title is part of UC Press's Voices Revived program, which commemorates University of California Press’s mission to seek out and cultivate the brightest minds and give them voice, reach, and impact. Drawing on a backlist dating to 1893, Voices Revived makes high-quality, peer-reviewed scholarship accessible once again using print-on-demand technology. This title was originally published in 1969.
LanguageEnglish
Release dateNov 15, 2023
ISBN9780520324145
Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art

Related to Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art

Related ebooks

Art For You

View More

Related articles

Reviews for Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars
0 ratings

0 ratings0 reviews

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

    Book preview

    Tradition and Creativity in Tribal Art - Daniel Biebuyck

    TRADITION AND

    CREATIVITY IN

    Published under the auspices of the Museum and Laboratories of

    Ethnic Arts and Technology University of California, Los Angeles

    TRADITION and

    CREATIVITY in

    Edited and with an Introduction by DANIEL P. BIEBUYCK

    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS

    BERKELEY AND LOS ANGELES 1969

    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS

    Berkeley and Los Angeles, California

    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS, LTD.

    London, England

    Copyright © 1969 by The Regents of the University of California

    Library of Congress Catalog Card Number: 69-12457

    Designed by James Mennick

    Printed in the United States of America

    Preface

    December 6,1965, through May 13,1966, the Museum and Laboratories of Ethnic Arts and Technology at the University of California, Los Angeles, organized an exhibition, Masterpieces from the Sir Henry Wellcome Collection at UCLA, in order to celebrate the gift from the Wellcome Trust in London of thousands of art objects and artifacts from all over the world. In conjunction with this exhibition, a lecture series and symposium were sponsored under the title Individual Creativity and Tribal Norms in Non-Western Arts. An international group of distinguished authorities on the so-called primitive arts participated in these proceedings from December, 1965, to May, 1966. The formal lectures, all richly illustrated with slides and film, were presented consecutively by R. Goldwater, W. Fagg, A. Gerbrands, R. Thompson, I. Bernal, and J. Guiart. A panel composed of R. Altman, W. Bascom, E. Carpenter, R. Sieber, and D. Biebuyck discussed some of the issues and problems raised during the lecture series.

    This book contains the revised lectures, together with the revised and expanded comments made by the members of the panel and the introduction written by the editor. The photographs included in the book represent a small selection of the material that originally illustrated the lectures. With the exception of the essays contributed by R. Thompson and W. Bascom, the original sequence in which both lectures and comments were presented has been respected. In the course of the revision process, R. Thompson’s paper developed into the full-scale artistic biography of one Nigerian artist. This longer contribution has been placed at the end of the lecture series as an example of a thus far rare case study in the literature on the meaning and development of a single artist’s oeuvre. W. Bascom’s contributions to the panel discussions are presented here as a self-contained essay. The editor acknowledges the support of the Ford Foundation and the UCLA Committee on International and Comparative Studies. He is also grateful to Miss B. Jones, Mrs. V. Dawson, and Mr. R. Abbott, all at the University of Delaware, for the work contributed in the editing and final presentation of the manuscript.

    D. B.

    Photographie Credits

    I. Bandi, Paris, plates 50 to 68

    British Museum, London, plates 13,14,24

    L. K. Carroll, Lagos, plate 20

    W. Fagg, London, plates 16 to 19 and 21 to 23

    Wilson Perkins Foss IV, Yale, plate 94

    Institute of African Studies, University of Ghana, Legón, plate 97

    Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia, Mexico City, plates

    33,41,42,44,46,49

    Mark Kinnaman, Yale, plate 86

    W. Moore, Los Angeles, plate 25

    Musée National des Arts Africains et Océaniens, Paris, plates 50 to 68

    Museo Nacional de Antropología, Mexico City, plates 38 to 40, 43,

    45,47,48

    Museum of Primitive Art, New York, plates 1 to 12

    Alex Nicoloff, Berkeley, plate 79

    Nigerian Museum, Lagos, plates 15,87

    vìi viii Photographic Credits

    Eugene R. Prince, Berkeley, plates 69 to 78

    Rijksmuseum voor Volkenkunde, Leiden, plates 26 to 37

    Robert H. Lowie Museum of Anthropology, Berkeley, plates 69

    to 79

    Roy Sieber, Bloomington, plate 96

    Smithsonian Institution, Washington, D.C., plate 86

    Robert F. Thompson, Yale, plates 80 to 85, 87 to 93, and 95

    Charles Uht, New York, plates 1 to 8 and 10 to 12

    Raymond Wielgus, Chicago, plate 9

    Contributors

    ALTMAN, RALPH C. Late Lecturer in the Department of Art, and Director, Museum and Laboratories of Ethnic Arts and Technology, University of California, Los Angeles. Publications include Comments on Studying Ethnological Art in Current Anthropology, North American Indian Arts in the Encyclopedia of World Art, and numerous exhibition catalogues.

    BASCOM, WILLIAM. Professor of Anthropology, and Director, Robert H. Lowie Museum of Anthropology, University of California, Berkeley. Field experience in Nigeria, Cuba, Caroline Islands, North America. Publications include The Sociological Role of the Yoruba Cult-Group, Continuity and Change in Africa (with M. J. Herskovits), Handbook of West African Art (with P. Gebauer), and African Arts.

    BERNAL, IGNACIO. Professor of Anthropology, and Director, Museo Nacional de Antropología, Mexico City. Field ex- perience in various sites in Mexico. Publications include Mexico before Cortez, Teotihuacan, Descubrimientos, Reconstrucciones, Bibliografía de Arqueiogía y Etnografía: Mesoamerica y Norte de Mexico, 1514—1960, and Urnas de Oaxaca (with A. Caso).

    BIEBUYCK, DANIEL. H. Rodney Sharp Professor of Anthropology and Sociology, University of Delaware. Field experience among Zoba, Bembe, Lega, Nyanga, and other ethnic groups of Congo/Kinshasa. Publications include African Agrarian Systems (ed.), Congo Tribes and Parties (with M. Douglas), The Mwindo Epic from the Banyanga (with K. Mateene), Anthologie de la littérature orale nyanga (with K. Mateene).

    CARPENTER, EDMUND. Research Professor, Schweitzer Program, Fordham University, Bronx, New York. Field experience in Canadian Arctic, Micronesia, Siberia, Borneo. Publications include Eskimo, Anerca: New Directions, Explorations in Communications, and We Wed Ourselves to the Mystery (forthcoming).

    FAGG, WILLIAM. Deputy Keeper of Ethnography in the British Museum. Field experience in Nigeria, Dahomey, Ghana, Ivory Coast, Sierra Leone, Congo/Kinshasa. Publications include The Sculpture of Africa (with E. Elisofon), Nigerian Images, African Art (with M. Plass), and Tribes and Forms in African Art.

    GERBRANDS, ADRIAN. Professor of Cultural Anthropology, Leiden University, Holland. Former Vice-Director of the Rijksmuseum voor Volkenkunde, Leiden. Field experience in New Guinea and New Britain. Publications include Art as an Element of Culture, Especially in Negro Africa, Wow-Ipits: Eight Asmat Wood-Carvers of New Guinea, and The Asmat of New Guinea: The Michael C. Rockefeller Expeditions (ed. and introduction).

    GOLDWATER, ROBERT. Professor of Fine Arts, New York University, and Chairman, Administrative Committee, Museum of Primitive Art, New York. Publications include Primitivism in Modern Art, Modern Art in your Life, Bambara Sculpture from the Western Sudan, and Senufo Sculpture from West Africa.

    GUIART, JEAN. Directeur d’Études (Religions Océaniennes), École Pratique des Hautes Études, Paris, and Chargé de Mission, Direction des Musées de France, Musée des Arts Africains et Océaniens, Paris. Field experience in New Caledonia, New Hebrides, Loyalty Islands, New Guinea. Publications include Océanie, Structure de la chefferie en Melanésie du Sud, and Nouvelles-Hébrides.

    SIEBER, ROY. Professor of Fine Arts, Indiana University, Bloomington. Field experience in Ghana and Nigeria. Publications include African Sculpture, Sculpture of Northern Nigeria, "The Visual Arts" in The African World, ed. R. A. Lystad (1965)-

    THOMPSON, ROBERT. Assistant Professor of Art History, Yale University. Field experience in Nigeria, Dahomey, Liberia, Cuba, and Brazil. Publications include Yoruba Artistic Criticism, in The Traditional Artist in African Society, ed. W. d’Azevedo (in press), Yoruba Beaded and Brass Crowns in Art and-Leadership in Africa, ed. D. Fraser and H. Cole (forthcoming), and An Aesthetic of the Cool: West African Dance in African Forum (1966).

    Contents

    Contents

    1. Introduction

    2. Judgments of Primitive Art, 1905-1965

    3. The African Artist

    4. The Concept of Style in Non-Western Art

    5. Individual Artistic Creativity in Pre-Columbian Mexico

    6. The Concept of Norm in the Art of Some Oceanian Societies

    7. Creativity and Style in African Art15

    8. Àbátàn: A Master Potter of the Ègbádò Yorùbá29

    9. Comments

    ROY SIEBER

    EDMUND CARPENTER

    Bibliography

    Index

    1. Introduction

    DANIEL BIEBUYCK

    IN THEIR many studies on morphology and distribution, students of primitive art have, more or less convincingly, established evidence to prove that there exist larger or smaller geographical areas within which, over a certain period of time, characteristic types of art objects, forms, designs, motifs, and style elements occur. They have, with varying results, tried to establish stylistic relationships among several such areas. Various descriptive concepts have been used to identify these areas of relatively uniform styles, such as stylistic area, art-culture area, style province, and tribal style. The specific terms by which these areas are labeled represent a heterogeneous set of geographical terms or tribal labels; sometimes they are borrowed from particular categories of objects, institutional complexes, or style elements. In addition, the literature abounds with dubious and imprecise ascriptions of objects that cannot be conveniently placed within the recognized stylistic areas. The methodology that underlies stylistic classifications is often lacking in consistency and rigidity. Some so-called stylistic areas, indeed, seem to be constructed merely on the readily observable occurrences of highly distinctive objects rather than on the clearcut isolation of precisely defined style elements. In many milieus, it has become a standard practice to speak about tribal style and tribal art. Surely the labels are convenient, but the identification of such concepts as tribal is uncertain. The reasons for this vagueness are obvious: cross-culturally valid definitions of these concepts are absent from the anthropological literature, and even in the better monographs one is often left in doubt as to the boundaries or the degree of uniformity of the cultures studied. Regardless of these difficulties, however, there is ample evidence to show that specific categories of art objects or specific art styles are often correlated not with whole cultures but with particular institutions, such as initiation systems, cults, voluntary associations, restricted belief systems, and myths. These institutions represent only one dimension of the entire culture; sometimes they have a local rather than a pantribal distribution; sometimes they are transtribal. For example, the literature on Africa occasionally makes reference to Bembe art and attempts to define Bembe style. The Bembe form a fairly closely knit cultural entity in the eastern Congo, but it is generally meaningless to speak about Bembe style. One finds among these people, first, an art (bichrome, wooden, bell masks facing in four directions) of the alunga association, which has a limited distribution in the southern part of Bembeland and extends into some adjoining northern Luba groups. One encounters, second, the hütende art (bi- or trichromatic, wooden, plank-board masks). The boys’ initiations, for which they are made, are held throughout Bembeland but are organized autonomously by localized maximal lineages, thus leaving scope for local specialization (which entails, among other things, the total absence of such masks in some parts of Bembeland). Third, there is the elanda art (masks made from hide or cloth, and studded with bead designs). Elanda is a semisecret association found only in some of the sixteen partially dispersed Bembe clans. There is the art of the punga association (small, wooden figurines) which is of Luba origin and was introduced into Bembeland within the last fifty years. There is the art of the bwami association (small, ivory figurines; rare, wooden face masks; and wooden animal figurines) which is so similar to the well-known art of the Lega that no writing on African art ever distinguishes one from the other. In addition, there are other art objects (wooden figurines) carved in styles reminiscent of the northern Luba, which are made in Bembeland by small, partially submerged groups of other than Bembe origin.

    In defining styles, the literature tends to focus on basic similarities and to ignore the significant differences that are easily discovered when one compares the various known pieces of a given class of objects found in a restricted area and within a closely knit cultural entity. Sometimes these differences and variations are just acknowledged; sometimes they are accounted for under such labels as substyles, local or village styles, schools, and much more rarely, at least for large parts of the primitive world, they are ascribed to different style periods. For our purpose, it is necessary to investigate further some of the factors underlying these differences and variations. In many areas of the world, artists of a single culture express themselves in a wide variety of material media (wood, bark, cane, fiber, rawhide, stone, clay, iron, bronze, gold, silver, ivory, bone, and others). Sometimes several of these materials may functionally be substituted for one another; in other instances, art objects made out of different materials are destined to serve the purposes of different segments of the population or of diverse artusing institutions. Sometimes artists are used to working in different materials; in many cases, they are more specialized, restricting themselves to artwork in a single medium. Different style traditions in the same society may or may not be linked with the handling of these diverse media (ivory carving against wood carving, or brass casting against wood carving) (e.g., Fagg and Plass, pp. 18, 21—25). Thus, in descriptions of style the use of different media with correlated techniques and traditions must be taken into consideration as a possible source for style differences. In many regions both two- and three-dimensional art are produced concurrently. As Schmitz (1956, p. 113) has pointed out, these two categories of technique create different problems and possibilities and cannot be intermixed in a general characterization. Basic differences in style are linked with these two categories of activity, and recourse to such concepts as substyle or style period is often unnecessary in this respect. There are many societies where certain types of objects are always made by men, according to what one could call male style traditions, in contrast with other objects that are women’s specialities. Among the American Northwest Coast Indians, the arts of weaving and basketry are practiced only by women, whereas painting and sculpture in wood and stone are done exclusively by men. Inverarity (1967, p. 45) finds that discrete style traditions are associated with this type of division of labor. Moreover, the time factor is a significant element in the assessment of stylistic difference. In any culture there is constant change going on, sometimes slow and uneven, often affecting certain spheres of activity more than others. Our documentation of the chronological distribution of artworks produced by separate cultures is, in general, very poor, and many of the collected works of art extend over only a short span of time. Yet, this factor of changing values, taste, aesthetic criteria, and needs, combined with changing influences, is necessarily a potent element in determining the gradual or consistent occurrence of variations.

    Other significant factors in variation become apparent if we restrict ourselves to art objects made in a single medium by artists of the same sex, and if we assume that all products under consideration fall within a very limited period of time. Different contexts of purpose and usage may impose different kinds of demand on the artists. Objects such as a Bambara mask made for the flankuru society have little in common with the komo masks among the same people. This very fact may, in turn, affect the number of artists operating or specializing at any given time in any given society. Functional purposes, volume of demand, number of artists can, in one way or another, enhance or lessen the possibilities for variation. Thus, the needs for conventional objects may be strictly circumscribed in some cases and flexible in others. Excessive demand for specific objects may lead to mass production, copying, and the acceptance by society of many mediocre artists. Invariably, societies are divided into many kinds of subgroupings, such as local kinship units, distinct political entities, ritual communities, age groups, and voluntary associations. The various subgroupings, although participating in a basic common culture, do not necessarily form common action groups; each may represent a self-contained unit, have its own set of specialized values, preferences, and action patterns, and possess its own interpretation of the common culture. In the literature on art, unfortunately, emphasis on this aspect of variation has often gone not much further than the so-called subtribe, the village, or the loosely defined district. Without indulging in cultural- historical speculation, there is also enough evidence available for many parts of the world that so-called homogeneous societies are composed of various substrata of peoples and of many incorporated groups of diverse ethnic origin. Although participating in an overall common culture, these component entities have steadfastly maintained or developed distinctive patterns and traditions in their technology and art. Finally, the art-producing societies do not exist in vacuo; isolation, self-containment, and self-sufficiency are relative concepts. There are many examples of wide-sweeping reciprocal influences that different peoples have exercised on one another’s art traditions. Some groups have shown more receptivity and creative originality in the process of borrowing than others that were either resilient or slavish in imitating. Whatever the case may be, in any society some component groups are more exposed to outside influences and eager to incorporate some of the extraneous art elements than others. It accounts in part for the widely observed spread across tribal boundaries of institutional complexes such as associations, initiations, cults, and their correlated art traditions.

    Beyond these many factors that contribute to the variations within culturally well-circumscribed groups, we have to consider the artists, themselves, as individuals, as members of particular schools, as proponents of local traditions, conventions, and systems of thought. Ultimately, most works of art are created and shaped by individuals, whether or not they are helped in some phases of their work by pupils or by other artists, and even though the patinaed finish or thick coating of their work may be the product of several anonymous generations of users. Some authors speculate about the absence of the concept artist in most primitive societies. There is no equivalent for art either, yet nobody doubts that primitive societies have produced objects that are pleasing and that strike one as beautiful. Obviously, these objects are not sheer replicas of one another, but works made by gifted individuals who in Malraux’s (1953, pp. 310, 416), words create forms and do not merely reproduce them. These artists then, have different personalities, different skills and proficiencies; they differ in age and maturity and are trained and steeped in local traditions. Some are highly specialized in one technique, others are versed in several crafts; some work publicly, others in secrecy; some work with models, others only with mental images and dreams. Some work in ateliers in the company of other artists with whom they can compare their works, under the direction of their patrons by whom they can be guided and of the larger public by whom they can be criticized. Other artists work privately and avoid or ignore criticism. Some work only when commissioned to make carvings; others create more freely when they feel an inclination to do so. Undoubtedly, whatever the stringencies and conventions of style, purpose and expectation, the individual element is a powerful factor in explaining differences. Artists necessarily differ in training, in skill and technical proficiency, in maturity and social position, and in personality. Society can impose upon its artists a certain objective subject matter and style but the artist himself has his own personal conception of the subject matter, a particular feeling for the style, and a certain technique in executing the form.

    The feasibility of the expression of individual taste, skill, and temperament in artistic productivity is a much debated question in regard to communities that focus heavily on corporate solidarity and collective action, and doubts are raised as to whether or not it is possible to speak about creative originality and conscious innovation. Surprisingly little work has been done in depth on the many aspects of this problem. Yet, the first attempts at unraveling some of its elements date back to the twenties and thirties in studies written by Firth (1925,1936) on the Maori carver and on New Guinea, Bunzel (1928) on the Pueblo potters, O'Neale (1932) on Yurok- Karok basket weavers, Himmelheber (1935, 1938) on West African and Eskimo artists, Herskovits (1934b, 1938) on Dahomey, Griaule (1938) on Dogon, Vandenhoute (1948) on Dan.¹ Some authors, like Firth, discussed the personality, the social position, the method of training of the artist, and the place of his work. An initiation ceremony was performed over the Maori artist which claimed to render him apt to receive instruction and "fixed the learning firmly in his mind. … Innovations were not permitted; mistakes were aitua (evil omens)" (Firth, 1925, p. 283). Others, like Bunzel, were involved with local and individual variation, its character and causes. Bunzel learned to distinguish the work of several potters in San Ildefonso, Acoma, and Hopi; the distinctions were more often a matter of mastery of technique than of style. She found more emphasis on originality and individualism in some Pueblo cultures than in others (Bunzel, 1928, pp. 62—68). Originality was apparent in two spheres: in minor, distinguishing, technical characteristics, such as texture of paste or use of color; in the highly distinctive treatment of form and of decorative elements (Bunzel, 1938, p. 566). In an unsurpassed analysis of typology, ethno-aesthetics, standards for excellence in the choice of materials, design arrangements, pattern placing, size and proportions of baskets, O'Neale (1932, passim) stated that within a compact body of established traditions choices are possible and alternates sought for certain portions of the basketry work more than for others. The rules for learning the craft, for selecting and using materials, for form, proportion, and design were rigid, but choice-making was permitted, for example, in the tone and rearrangement of color or in the selection of material for a design element. O'Neale concluded (1932, p. 165): Far from being deadened by a craft in which so much is reduced to conformity, the women of the two tribes have developed an appreciation of quality, design-to-space relationships, and effective color disposition, which are discriminating and genuine.

    Aspects of individual style and creativity are, of course, treated in recent works. Some of the more elaborate discussions are to be found in Himmelheber (1960), Smith (1961), Fischer (1963), Fagg (1951, 1958, 1963), Gerbrands (1967), Carroll (1967), Forge (1967), d’Azevedo (1966), Goodale and Koss (1967). Many other studies have index entries for artist, but often the so-called artist is unnamed and treated in abstract and nonspecific terms.2 All in all, little attention has been paid to the problem, and the reasons for it are numerous. Most objects that form the subject of art studies were torn away from their social context by untrained foreigners who were barely interested in the products themselves, let alone their makers. Most anthropologists have shown an enduring disregard for detailed field studies on the aesthetic dimensions of primitive societies. Most students of primitive art have been satisfied with classification of products into so-called homogeneous styles and substyles, with the distribution of motifs, and with general stereotypes about their meaning and function. Emphasis has been placed only on select aspects of the total artistic activity or on seleet categories of objects, leading to an unfortunate compartmentalization of otherwise closely integrated artistic activities.

    In most general handbooks on primitive art the authors are concerned with the principles of individual style variation and creative freedom, accepting, sometimes with several reservations, that they do exist.

    Wingert (1965, pp. 15—17) claims that the artist was not so completely restrained by his society and his patrons as to be a mere copyist; he could endow the traditional forms with his own interpretation and insight and In this controlled material he created, following traditional patterns, his own renderings of the requisite forms.

    Fraser (1962, p. 22) writes: Not only does the primitive artist strive to be understood, but also every step of his selection and training forces him in a traditional direction. Buehler, Barrow, and Mountford (1962, p. 42) summarize their point of view as follows: Each work of art is therefore the expression not merely of individual experiences, sensations, and values, but also of the influences and attitudes of the culture concerned. The message of a work thus expresses cultural as well as personal attributes.

    Himmelheber (1960, p. 23) also takes a moderate stand: Der Künstler hält sich ziemlich streng an die ihm von diesem Stammes- stil vorgeschriebenen Formen. He stresses, however, the marked differences with which four Guro artists render an elephant head, and demonstrates how various Senufo artists represent in different degrees of stylization the traditional women’s coiffure in the form of a bird’s head and beak (Himmelheber, 1960, p. 64, figs. 52, 53).

    At this stage of very limited knowledge on individual style and freedom many of our questions must await an answer because so few specific data are available. But the questions relating to the general nature of the limitations imposed upon artists by different societies can be fruitfully pursued.

    It is true, as Read (1961, p. 124) points out, that a work of art is essentially individualistic in origin, and that from artist to artist there are differences in personality and temperament. It is equally true that the artist is deeply steeped in his milieu, versed in its values, eager to be in conformity with them and to be acceptable by his group. The artist is usually not a solitary person, cut off from his milieu by his own will, engaged in a full-time pursuit of the aesthetic, involved merely in a world of forms which he creates for himself. Firth’s statement that the artist-craftsman is only a part-time or leisure-time worker in this activity generally holds true (Firth, 1951, p. 172). Moreover, the artist is himself frequently an active participant in the rites and ceremonies in which his products are used. All this implies that, as Firth remarks (ibid., p. 173), the artist is not divorced from his public and that the primitive artist and his public share essentially the same set of values. Yet, in societies where some of the art is used in the highly esoteric contexts of initiations into closed associations, the artist may not be familiar with the ultimate meaning and destination of his products. The artist has a message to convey, a concept or belief to sustain with his work of art; he must, therefore, be readable and understandable to his public. But again this public, of which he is himself a part, is highly skilled in reading symbolic messages; it may cultivate, as Leach notes (1954, p. 29), a faculty for making and understanding ambiguous statements. It is my experience among the Lega of the Congo that this public is also flexible and creative in the interpretation of symbolic messages conveyed through the art forms. We may conclude that there is a close, reciprocal bond between the artist and his community, which both compels him to do certain things and

    Enjoying the preview?
    Page 1 of 1