Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

Only $11.99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2: Social and Emotional Development
The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2: Social and Emotional Development
The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2: Social and Emotional Development
Ebook2,717 pages33 hours

The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2: Social and Emotional Development

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars

()

Read preview

About this ebook

In the past fifty years, scholars of human development have been moving from studying change in humans within sharply defined periods, to seeing many more of these phenomenon as more profitably studied over time and in relation to other processes. The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2: Social and Emotional Development presents the study of human development conducted by the best scholars in the 21st century. Social workers, counselors and public health workers will receive coverage of the social and emotional aspects of human change across the lifespan.
LanguageEnglish
PublisherWiley
Release dateDec 14, 2010
ISBN9781118026519
The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2: Social and Emotional Development

Related to The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2

Related ebooks

Psychology For You

View More

Related articles

Reviews for The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars
0 ratings

0 ratings0 reviews

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

    Book preview

    The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2 - Wiley

    001

    Table of Contents

    Title Page

    Copyright Page

    Preface

    A BRIEF HISTORY OF THE LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE

    THE VIEW FROM THE HILLS

    CONCLUSIONS

    THE GOALS OF THIS HANDBOOK

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    REFERENCES

    Contributors

    CHAPTER 1 - Introduction

    AN OVERVIEW OF THIS VOLUME

    CONCLUSIONS

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 2 - Neurobiological Bases of Social Behavior across the Life Span

    INTRODUCTION AND CONTEXT

    SOCIAL BEHAVIOR IN CONTEXT

    THE AUTONOMIC NERVOUS SYSTEM AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

    POLYVAGAL THEORY: A NEW BIOBEHAVIORAL CONCEPTUALIZATION OF THE AUTONOMIC ...

    THE NEURAL BASIS OF SOCIAL ENGAGEMENT AND COMMUNICATION

    INTEGRATED COMPONENTS OF THE SOCIAL ENGAGEMENT SYSTEM AND BEHAVIORAL REGULATION

    THE AUTONOMIC NERVOUS SYSTEM AS A NEURAL PLATFORM FOR SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

    NEUROCHEMICAL FACTORS COORDINATE AUTONOMIC STATE AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

    SUMMARY

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 3 - The Development of Emotion Regulation: A Neuropsychological Perspective

    WHAT IS EMOTION REGULATION AND WHEN DOES IT OCCUR?

    NEURAL INVESTIGATIONS OF EMOTION REGULATION

    HOW DOES THE NORMATIVE DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTION REGULATION INTERACT WITH ...

    DEVELOPMENTAL VERSUS INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN THE NEURAL UNDERPINNINGS OF ...

    WHY DO NEGATIVE EMOTION AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY INCREASE IN ADOLESCENCE?

    WHY DO OLDER ADULTS EXPERIENCE MORE POSITIVE EMOTION?

    CONCLUSIONS

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 4 - Dynamic Integration of Emotion and Cognition: Equilibrium ...

    ENERGETICS, STRUCTURE, AND EQUILIBRIUM IN LIFE-SPAN EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

    LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTIONAL SYSTEMS

    CONCLUSION AND OUTLOOK

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 5 - Self-Regulation across the Life Span

    SELF-REGULATION AND ACTION THEORY

    AN ORGANIZING HEURISTIC

    SUMMARY OF DEVELOPMENT

    SUMMARY OF THE HEURISTIC

    LIMITATIONS AND BURNING QUESTIONS

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 6 - Self and Identity across the Life Span

    TRADITIONS OF INQUIRY

    THE SELF AS ACTOR

    THE SELF AS AGENT

    THE SELF AS AUTHOR

    CONCLUSION

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 7 - Temperament and Personality through the Life Span

    CONCEPTS

    CONSISTENCY AND CHANGE IN TEMPERAMENT AND PERSONALITY

    THE LITERATURE ON CONSISTENCY AND CHANGE IN TEMPERAMENT AND PERSONALITY

    FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TO TEMPERAMENT AND PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT

    LINKS BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND ADJUSTMENT

    CONCLUSION

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 8 - Life-Span Perspectives on Positive Personality Development in ...

    A LIFE-SPAN APPROACH TO PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT

    PERSONALITY ADJUSTMENT AND GROWTH

    EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE ON PERSONALITY GROWTH AND ADJUSTMENT TRAJECTORIES

    CONTEXTS FACILITATING POSITIVE PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT: UNFOLDING PERSONALITY PLASTICITY

    CONCLUSION AND OUTLOOK

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 9 - Coping across the Life Span

    COPING: A BRIEF PRIMER

    GENERAL OVERVIEW OF COPING BY LIFE PHASE

    TOWARD A LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 10 - Gendered Behavior across the Life Span

    GENDER DEVELOPMENT BEFORE BIRTH: EARLY HORMONAL INFLUENCES

    SEX DIFFERENCES IN HUMAN BEHAVIOR

    GENDERED BEHAVIOR IN CHILDHOOD

    CORE GENDER IDENTITY

    SEXUAL ORIENTATION

    COGNITION

    EMOTION

    THE LIFE SPAN AFTER CHILDHOOD

    CONCLUSIONS

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 11 - Intimate Relationships across the Life Span

    BASIC QUESTIONS: LOVE AND PAIRBONDING

    ADOLESCENT ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS

    THE RELEVANCE OF EMERGING ADULTHOOD

    SETTLING INTO ADULTHOOD: SATISFACTION AND STABILITY

    RELATIONSHIP THREATS

    INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE

    WHEN ALL EFFORTS FAIL: DIVORCE

    RELATIONSHIPS, HEALTH, AND PSYCHOBIOLOGY

    EMERGING PERSPECTIVES ON A UNIQUE POPULATION: SAME-SEX COUPLES

    PROMISING NEW DIRECTIONS IN RELATIONSHIP RESEARCH

    CONCLUSION

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 12 - Convoys of Social Relations: Integrating Life-Span and Life-Course Perspectives

    SOCIAL RELATIONS: THE CONVOY MODEL

    CONVOY MODEL: UPDATE

    LIFE-SPAN AND LIFE-COURSE PERSPECTIVES

    DISENGAGEMENT THEORY AND ACTIVITY THEORY

    SOCIOEMOTIONAL SELECTIVITY THEORY

    SOCIAL INPUT MODEL

    EXCHANGE AND RECIPROCITY MODEL

    SUBSTITUTION MODELS

    FUNCTIONAL MODELS

    ROLE THEORY

    THE UNIQUE CONTRIBUTION OF THE PATTERN-CENTERED APPROACH WITH LIFE-SPAN SOCIAL ...

    OVERVIEW OF PATTERN-CENTERED APPROACH

    TRANSITIONS DURING CHILDHOOD AND THEIR EFFECT ON SOCIAL NETWORK TYPES

    TRANSITIONS IN ADULTHOOD AND THEIR EFFECT ON SOCIAL NETWORK TYPES

    CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN NETWORK TYPES

    FUTURE RESEARCH, SUMMARY, AND CONCLUSIONS

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 13 - Achievement Motives and Goals: A Developmental Analysis Goals:

    COMPETENCE AND MOTIVATION: THE CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATION OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION

    ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVES

    MOVING FROM ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVES TO ACHIEVEMENT GOALS

    ACHIEVEMENT GOALS

    CONCLUDING STATEMENTS

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 14 - Developmental Psychopathology

    HISTORICAL UNDERPINNINGS OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

    WHAT IS DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY?

    DEFINITIONAL PARAMETERS AND PRINCIPLES OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

    CHILD MALTREATMENT: A MULTILEVEL PERSPECTIVE

    ILLUSTRATIVE DISORDERS FROM A DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY PERSPECTIVE

    CONCLUSION

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 15 - Developing Civic Engagement within a Civic Context

    TRENDS IN CIVIC ENGAGEMENT

    CIVIC ENGAGEMENT IN AN ECOLOGICAL MODEL OF DEVELOPMENT

    INTRAINDIVIDUAL FACTORS RELATED TO CIVIC INVOLVEMENT

    THE ROLES OF VARIOUS CONTEXTS IN DEVELOPING CIVIC ENGAGEMENT

    PUTTING IT ALL TOGETHER: NON-COLLEGE-BOUND YOUTH AS AN EXAMPLE

    CONCLUSIONS

    REFERENCES

    CHAPTER 16 - Religious and Spiritual Development across the Life Span

    DEFINING RELIGIOUS AND SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT

    THEORIES: EXPLAINING RELIGIOUS-SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT

    DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS AND RELIGIOUS-SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT

    PROBLEMATIC RELIGIOUS-SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT

    CONCLUDING REMARKS

    REFERENCES

    Author Index

    Subject Index

    001

    This book is printed on acid-free paper. 002

    Copyright © 2010 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved.

    Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey.

    Published simultaneously in Canada.

    No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008.

    Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a professional where appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages.

    This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. If legal, accounting, medical, psychological or any other expert assistance is required, the services of a competent professional person should be sought.

    Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are often claimed as trademarks. In all instances where John Wiley & Sons, Inc. is aware of a claim, the product names appear in initial capital or all capital letters. Readers, however, should contact the appropriate companies for more complete information regarding trademarks and registration.

    For general information on our other products and services please contact our Customer Care Department within the United States at (800) 762-2974, outside the United States at (317) 572-3993 or fax (317) 572-4002.

    Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in print may not be available in electronic books.

    For more information about Wiley products, visit our web site at www.wiley.com.

    Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data:

    The handbook of life-span development / editor-in-chief, Richard M. Lerner.

    p. cm.

    Includes author and subject indexes.

    ISBN 978-0-470-39011-5 (v. 1 : cloth); ISBN 978-0-470-39012-2 (v. 2 : cloth); ISBN 978-0-470-39013-9 (set: cloth); 978-0-470-63433-2 (ebk); 978-0-470-63434-9 (ebk); 978-0-470-63435-6 (ebk)

    1. Developmental psychology. 2. Maturation (Psychology) 3. Aging-Psychological aspects. I. Lerner, Richard M.

    BF713.H3648 2010

    155-dc22

    2009049300

    Preface

    Until the early 1960s, the field of human development was dominated by either descriptions of the behavioral or psychological phenomena presumptively unfolding as a consequence of genetically controlled timetables of maturational change (e.g., see the chapters by Hess and by McClearn in the third edition of the Handbook of Child Psychology; Mussen, 1970) or by descriptions of the behaviors presumptively elicited in response to stimulation encountered over the course of early life experiences (e.g., see the chapters by Stevenson or by White in the same edition of the Handbook). Framed within a Cartesian dualism that split nature from nurture (Overton, 2006), developmental science focused in the main on the generic human being (Emmerich, 1968) and on the earliest years of life or, at most, the years surrounding the stages of pubertal change. These periods were regarded as the portions of ontogeny in which the fundamental processes of human development emerged and functioned to shape the subsequent course of human life (Brim & Kagan, 1980).

    Today, the study of development has evolved from a field embedded within the domain of developmental psychology to an area of scholarship labeled developmental science (Bornstein & Lamb, 2005, 2010; Magnusson & Stattin, 1998, 2006). Substantively, developmental science is a field that conceptualizes the entire span of human life as potentially involving developmental change. The possibility of developmental change across life exists because the basic process of development is seen as involving mutually influential relations between an active organism and a changing, multilevel ecology, a relation represented as individual ↔ context relations (Lerner, 2006). These relations provide the fundamental impetus to systematic and successive changes across the life span (Brandtstädter, 1998; Overton, 1973, 2003; Lerner, 2006).

    Thus, the contemporary study of human development involves placing postmodern, relational models at the cutting edge of theoretical and empirical interest (Overton, 2006). These models consider all levels of organization—from the inner biological through the physical ecological, cultural, and historical—as involved in mutually influential relationships across the breadth of the entire life course (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Riegel, 1975, 1976). Variations in time and place constitute vital sources of systematic changes across ontogeny—even into the 10th and 11th decades of life—and, as such, human life is variegated and characterized by intraindividual change and interindividual differences (Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 2006; Elder, Modell, & Parke, 1993). Accordingly, because ontogenetic change is embodied in its relation to time and place (Overton, 2006), contemporary developmental science regards the temporality represented by historical changes as imbued in all levels of organization, as coacting integratively, and as providing a potential for this systematic change—for plasticity—across the life span.

    In short, as a consequence of the relational coactions of changes at levels of organization ranging from the biological and the psychological and behavioral to the sociocultural, designed and natural physical ecological, and through the historical (see Gottlieb, 1997; Overton, 2006), processes of development are viewed in contemporary developmental science through a theoretical and empirical lens that extends the study of change across the human ontogenetic span and, as well, through generational and historical time (Elder, 1998; Elder et al., 1993). The variations in the actions of individuals on their contexts and contexts on individuals integratively propel and texture the course of life (Baltes, Freund, & Li, 2005; Brandtstädter, 2006; Freund & Baltes, 2000 Freund, Li, & Baltes, 1999). As a result, the breadth of the life span and all levels of organization within the ecology of human development must be considered to fully describe, explain, and optimize the course of intraindividual change and of interindividual differences in such change (Baltes et al., 2006; Baltes, Reese, & Nesselroade, 1977).

    There exist both historical (Baltes, 1979, 1983; Cairns & Cairns, 2006), philosophical, and theoretical (Lerner, 1984; Overton, 1973, 1975, 2006) accounts of the nature and bases of the evolution of developmental science. These accounts document that the field changed from one dominated by psychological, environmental, or biological reductionist, split, and age-period-restricted conceptions of human development processes to become a field focused on relational, systems, and life-span developmental models. As Edwin G. Boring (1950, p. ix) noted, Hermann Ebbinghaus once remarked that psychology has a long past, but only a short history. In many ways, the same statement may be made about the evolution of the life-span view of human development.

    A BRIEF HISTORY OF THE LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE

    The life-span ideas that we today summarize as the lifespan perspective began to emerge in the United States in the mid-1960s and early 1970s. However, the conception that was forwarded was neither a newly created perspective of human development nor the only extant conception of lifespan development present at the time (or even now). To a great extent, the history of the emergence and refinement of this life-span perspective arose through the discussions at, and the subsequent edited volumes derived from, a series of conferences held in the late 1960s and early to mid-1970s at the University of West Virginia in Morgantown. The conferences were held under the aegis of the Department of Psychology, which was chaired by K. Warner Schaie. As Baltes (1979, 1983) and Cairns and Cairns (2006) explained, the roots of the approach that began to crystallize in Morgantown can be traced to 19th-century scholarship in the United States and to philosophical ideas forwarded about 200 years earlier in Europe. In turn, although there was a different concentration of empirical attention paid to successive portions of human ontogeny (with most attention paid to infancy and childhood and comparatively less work devoted to studying people as they aged into the adolescent and adult periods), many U.S. developmental psychologists might argue that the study of human life within the social and behavioral sciences has always included a focus on behavior across the breadth of ontogeny. Despite the fact that the label life-span developmental psychology was not used, there was, at least since G. Stanley Hall’s work on senescence (1922), attention paid in some way to periods of life beyond childhood and adolescence.

    Nevertheless, despite any claims made that developmental science has been concerned for at least 90 years with development across life, the structure and function of academic work would contradict such assertions. Indeed, it was not until the 1970s and early 1980s, when the term life-span developmental psychology became popular in developmental psychology (e.g., Baltes & Schaie, 1973; Goulet & Baltes, 1970; Nesselroade & Reese, 1973), that many departments began offering life-span development courses.

    Moreover, most people involved in teaching these courses were trained in infancy, childhood, or, in a few cases, adolescence. They taught what they knew most and, as such, courses were mostly modestly revised child development courses with a few lectures at the end of the course devoted to adult development and aging. The textbooks that were written for use in these courses—in the main by colleagues also not involved in the study of developmental change across the breadth of human ontogeny—reinforced the approach taken by classroom instructors. Texts were in fact slightly revised child development books with a couple of chapters (and in some cases only one chapter) added about adult development and aging; at the time, the late adult years were still not seen as a part of the process of development. In short, authors wrote these books, and publishers structured them, to meet the needs of the instructors, which were to present mostly infancy and childhood, perhaps a chapter on adolescence (puberty, the identity crisis, and problem behaviors were in large part the focus of such chapters) and, finally, adulthood and aging.

    When the work of the key scholars studying the latter portion of the life span, for instance, developmental scientists such as Paul B. Baltes, James Birren, John R. Nesselroade, or K. Warner Schaie, among others, were cited, this scholarship was reduced to the fact that these scholars had promoted the idea of studying development across the life span. Therefore, because the existence of such life-span work required some treatment in the textbook, the author of the textbook would commence to present such topics as the life tasks of adulthood (as discussed, for instance, by Robert Havighurst, 1951), some ideas about intellectual decline with aging, and death and dying. What was wrong with such approaches to life-span human development?

    THE VIEW FROM THE HILLS

    These early textbooks in life-span development missed the points being made by the scholars who were gathering in Morgantown, West Virginia, and who were shaping quite a different approach to the study of development across the life span. The work of Baltes, Schaie, Nesselroade, and other major contributors (e.g., scholars such as Nancy Datan, Lawrence Goulet, Willis Overton, Hayne Reese, and Klaus Riegel) to the foundation of the approach to life-span development that has evolved to frame developmental science had, at best, been misinterpreted and, at worst (and in fact most of the time), trivialized. What, then, were the ideas being developed by the scholars meeting in Morgantown? Answering this question is central to the present work: The ideas that began to be developed in the West Virginia hills provide the foundation of the scholarship represented in this Handbook. Indeed, one cannot overestimate the impact on developmental science theory and methodology of the books that were derived from the West Virginia conferences (Baltes & Schaie, 1973; Datan & Reese, 1977; Goulet & Baltes, 1970; Nesselroade & Reese, 1973) and from a subsequent series of annual advances volumes, Life-Span Development and Behavior, first edited by Paul Baltes (1978); then by Baltes and Orville G. Brim, Jr. (1979, 1980, 1982, 1983, 1984); by Baltes, David L. Featherman, and Richard M. Lerner (1986, 1988a, 1988b, 1990); and by Featherman, Lerner, and Marion Perlmutter (1992, 1994). These works pushed the study of human development beyond the split and reductionist conceptual boundaries to which the field had accommodated (Overton, 2006).

    As Baltes (1987; Baltes et al., 2006) explained, the life-span view of human development was associated with the integration of a set of ideas, each of which could be found as stand-alone concepts within the developmental literature; however, when taken as an integrated or, in fact, a fused, or relational, whole, these ideas changed the conceptual landscape of the field. As such, the set of concepts introduced in the late 1960s and early 1970s at the University of West Virginia life-span development conferences embodied an approach to the study of the human life span that stood in sharp contrast to the simplistic, additive approach to development found in the early textbooks and associated courses in life-span development. Although these West Virginian ideas have of course evolved across the ensuing decades and were refined and extended by the their originators and by those who were influenced or trained by them, the fundamental character of these ideas remains the same and constitutes at least a sea change, if not a true paradigm shift, in the nature of thinking about human development.

    First, development was seen as a process, one that began at conception and continued through the end of life. For example, developmental processes were conceptualized as involving systematic and successive changes in the organization of relations within and across the levels of organization comprising the ecology of human development (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Lerner, 2002, 2006; Overton, 1973, 1978, 2006; Overton & Reese, 1981; Reese & Overton, 1970). There were both qualitatively and quantitatively continuous and discontinuous facets of this process. Accordingly, mutidirectionality of development (increases, decreases, curvilinearity, smooth or abrupt change, etc.) were all possible forms for a developmental process, and the shape or form of a developmental trajectory for an individual or group was a matter of theory-predicated empirical inquiry (Wohlwill, 1973).

    In addition, because variation in the form of developmental trajectories may occur for different people (e.g., people who vary with regard to age, sex, race, birth cohort, etc.), living in different settings, or in different historical eras, developmental process may take a different form at different points in ontogeny, generational time, or history across individuals or groups. Thus, diversity—with regard to within-person changes but also to differences between people in within-person change—rose to the level of substantive significance (as opposed to error variance) within the life-span view. For instance, as explained by Bornstein (1995), in regard to his specificity principle of infant development specific experiences at specific times exert specific effects over specific aspects of infant growth in specific ways (p. 21). In turn, a similar idea was advanced by Freund, Nikitin, and Ritter (2009), albeit one focused at the other end of the life span. Underscoring the importance of viewing the developmental process across the breadth of ontogeny, Freund et al. noted that a person’s development during a historical period of extended life expectancy is likely to have important implications for development during young and middle adulthood.

    Accordingly, although it would make sense from a life-span developmental perspective to study individuals and groups within (as well as across) ontogenetic age periods, an age period-specific focus should not be adopted because of the mistaken belief that the developmental process that occurs in childhood or adolescence is somehow a different developmental process than the one that occurs in adulthood and late adulthood. Rather, the life-span developmental scientist has the task of describing and explaining and, as noted again later in this preface, optimizing the form of such change across life. He or she must detail the ways in which changes within one period are derived from changes at earlier periods and affect changes at subsequent periods.

    As well, explanations of continuities and discontinuities across life, and of the form (the shape) of the developmental trajectory and of its rate of change, involve a very different theoretical frame than the ones that had been dominant in other approaches to the study of development (i.e., the split and reductionist approaches of past eras). As explained by Overton, both in his prior work (e.g., 1973, 2006) and in the chapter that introduces Volume 1 of the present Handbook, a relational metamodel frames the contemporary, cutting-edge study of human development within and across all portions of ontogeny. The use of this relational perspective emerged through the influence of Overton and other developmental scientists (e.g., Sameroff, 1975), including those studying infancy (e.g., Bornstein, 1995; Lamb, 1977; Lewis, 1972; Lewis & Rosenblum, 1974; Thelen & Smith, 1998), as well as through the contributions of comparative scientists (e.g., Gottlieb, 1997; Greenberg & Tobach, 1984; Kuo, 1976; Schneirla, 1957) and biologists (e.g., Novikoff, 1945a, 1945b; von Bertalanffy, 1933, 1968). This scholarship resulted in the elaboration of several variants of a developmental systems theoretical model of development (e.g., Ford & Lerner, 1992; Lerner, 2002); more recently, by explicitly incorporating a relational perspective, these models have been termed relational developmental systems models of development (Lerner, 2006; Lerner & Overton, 2008).

    Because it may seem counterintuitive that the scholarship of developmental scientists studying infancy was integral in the foundation of the life-span approach to human development, it is useful to illustrate briefly the contributions of such work to the life-span approach. The scholarship of Michael Lewis (e.g., 1972; Lewis & Lee-Painter, 1974) built on the insights of Bell (1968), about the potential presence in correlational data about socialization, of evidence for the bidirectional influences between parents and children. Lewis and his colleagues launched a program of research that provided a new, relational model of infant-parent interaction. In The Effect of the Infant on Its Caregiver (Lewis & Rosenblum, 1974), a volume that represents a watershed event in the history of the study of human development through the use of person↔context relational models, Lewis and colleagues argued that Not only is the infant or child influenced by its social, political, economic and biological world, but in fact the child itself influences its world in turn (Lewis & Rosenblum, 1974, p. xv) and maintained that "only through interaction can we study, without distortion, human behavior (Lewis & Lee-Painter, 1974, p. 21). Envisioning the relational, developmental systems models that would come to the fore in the study of human development a quarter century later, Lewis noted that a

    relational position not only requires that we deal with elements in interaction but also requires that we not consider the static quality of these interactions. Rather, it is necessary to study their flow with time….Exactly how this might be done is not at all clear. It may be necessary to consider a more metaphysical model, a circle in which there are neither elements nor beginnings/ends. (Lewis & Lee-Painter, 1974, pp. 46-47).

    Lewis’s scholarship fostered an intellectual climate among other infancy researchers that resulted in a re-conceptualization of phenomena of infant development within the sorts of relational, individual ↔ context relational models he forwarded. I noted earlier in this preface the contributions of Marc Bornstein (1995) in this regard. Here, too, however, we should point out another, foundational instance of the contributions of infancy researchers to the life-span perspective, found in the work of Michael Lamb. For instance, Lamb and his colleagues (e.g., Lamb, 1977; Lamb, Thompson, Gardner, & Charnov, 1985; Thompson & Lamb, 1986) approached the study of infant attachment within the context of the assumptions that (a) children influence their socializers and are not simply the receptive foci of socializing forces, (b) early social and emotional/personality development occurs in the context of a complex family system rather than only in the context of the mother-infant dyad, and (c) social and psychological development are not confined to infancy or childhood but involve a process that continues from birth to death (Lamb, 1978, p. 137; cf. Riley, 1979).

    Within this conceptual framework, Lamb and his colleagues found that prior interpretations of infant attachment, which included an emphasis on the formative significance of early experiences, a focus on unidirectional influences on the child, a tendency to view development within a narrow ecological context, and a search for universal processes of developmental change (Thompson & Lamb, 1986, p. 1), were less powerful in accounting for the findings of attachment research than an interpretation associated with person↔context relational models. Accordingly, in a review of attachment research conducted through the mid-1980s, Lamb and colleagues concluded that reciprocal organism-environment influences, developmental plasticity, individual patterns of developmental change and broader contextual influences on development can better help to integrate and interpret the attachment literature, and may also provide new directions for study (Thompson & Lamb, 1986, p. 1).

    Lamb’s work challenged the field of infancy to move the study of the early years of life beyond the use of narrow, split, or reductionist conceptions of the exclusive influences of heredity or early experiences or of simplistic views of proximal dyadic relationships acting in isolation from the fuller and richer ecology of human development. He provided instead a relational vision for the understanding of infancy as part of the entire life span, of the life-span development of the other people in the infant’s world, and of the complex set of infant ↔ social context relations as reciprocal exchanges in and with a multilevel and dynamic context (e.g., see Lamb, 1977; Lamb et al., 1985).

    In short, as exemplified by the work embedding the study of infancy within a life-span approach to human development, the theoretical approaches that emerged in the 1970s from the impetus given developmental science by the scholars gathering in the hills of West Virginia constituted an integrative approach to variables across the levels of organization within the relational developmental system. As such, these models rejected as counterfactual the split conceptions, theories, and metatheories that partitioned the sources of development into nature and nurture. Rejected as well were compromise views that, although admitting that both of these purportedly separate sources of influence were involved in development, used problematic (i.e., additive) conceptions of interaction (conceptualized much as are interaction terms in an analysis of variance or in other instantiations of the general linear model; see Gottlieb, Wahlsten, & Lickliter, 2006). Instead, the theoretical models promoted by the scholars contributing to the foundation and evolution of the life-span perspective stressed that the basic process of human development involves developmental regulations—that is, individual ↔ context relations that link all levels of the ecology of human development within a thoroughly integrated, or fused (Tobach & Greenberg, 1984), dynamic, relational system.

    For instance, these levels of the ecology involve ecological systems within the person (i.e., biosocial influences) or most proximal to him or her (what Bronfenbrenner [1979] termed the microsystem), extend to the set of contexts within which the individual interacts (the mesosystem; Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and to the systems in the ecology within which components of the mesosystem (e.g., parents) interact (e.g., the workplace) that may not directly involve the person but nevertheless may have an impact on him or her. In addition, the ecology includes the macrosystem —the broadest level of the ecology; the macrosystem influences all other systems and includes social policies, major intuitions of society (such as education, health care, and the economy), the designed and natural physical ecology, and, ultimately, history. As noted, this latter level of organization within the relational, integrated developmental system provides a temporal component for all other facets of the developmental system and creates the potential for systematic change, for plasticity, in individual ↔ context relations.

    One recent instantiation of such relational, developmental systems thinking in regard to the dynamics of the person’s exchanges with his or her ecology involves the work of Freund and colleagues in regard to the nature of the goals that individuals pursue within their changing context (e.g., Freund, 2007; Nikitin & Freund, 2008). Freund argues that the interplay of different levels of organization is important not only at the levels of person, family, and society (or other nested levels) but also for understanding the role of specific constructs—in this case, goals—for development. Extending action-theoretical concepts that have tended to view goals as primarily personal constructs, Freund explained that goals are located at multiple levels of the developmental system and that mutually influential relations among these levels need to be assessed to understand fully the nature and role of goals in human development. These levels involve social norms and expectations that inform about age-related opportunity structures and goal-relevant resources; personal beliefs about the appropriate timing and sequencing of goals; personal goals that are influenced by social norms, personal beliefs, the individual’s learning history, and external (e.g., social and physical environment) and internal (e.g., talent) resources; and nonconscious goals and motives that might be particularly influential in times of transition or in times of routine.

    CONCLUSIONS

    A relational developmental systems model of human development constitutes the approach to studying the life span that evolved from the ideas developed among the scholars gathering in the formative conferences at the University of West Virginia (Baltes & Schaie, 1973; Datan & Reese, 1977; Goulet & Baltes, 1970; Nesselroade & Reese, 1973). It is this dynamic, relational perspective that constitutes a radical departure from approaches that study nature, or nurture, or additive, linear (or even curvilinear) combinations (even if cast as interactions) between the two. Because of the emphasis on the dynamics of relations between the multiple levels of organization involved in the individual and the multiple levels of organization that are part of the ecology of human development, we need to adopt an ontogenetically all-encompassing—a fully life-span—approach to studying relational developmental processes. Such a conceptual frame is required if we are to understand the import of individual ↔ context relations for fostering specific changes, for a specific individual or group, living in a specific context, at a specific point in history (Bornstein, 1995). Clearly, such scholarship requires a multidisciplinary integrative (truly interdisciplinary) approach to the study of human development. Indeed, this need for collaboration across disciplines is in large part why developmental psychology has been transformed into developmental science (Lerner, 2006).

    In addition, the concepts of developmental regulation and plasticity integral to a developmental systems framing of the study of development across the life span have another quite important implication for the conduct of developmental science. These concepts in combination afford optimism about the possibility of finding or instantiating individual ↔ context relations that may increase the probability of positive developmental change. Indeed, the life-span approach to developmental science suggests that applications of relational, theory-predicated research findings (in the form of policies or programs) may promote more positive courses of development across life. Efforts to optimize the course of human life through the application of relationally framed developmental science provide, then, an opportunity to test developmental systems ideas about the impact of changes in individual ↔ context relations. As well, such applied efforts constitute a way for developmental scientists to contribute to the improvement of human life among diverse people and their settings. Ultimately, then, such applications of developmental science may contribute to the enhancement of social justice (Lerner & Overton, 2008).

    THE GOALS OF THIS HANDBOOK

    The innovative ideas associated with a developmental systems-framed approach to the study of life-span development is coupled with an admittedly ambitious agenda of basic and applied developmental science devoted to studying and optimizing processes of individual ↔ context relations across the life span. Nevertheless, despite the methodological complexities of adopting this relational approach to developmental science, the past 40 years have provided depth and breadth of empirical evidence in support of the usefulness of these ideas in framing methodologically rigorous and substantively significant developmental science.

    As such, one might think that numerous scholarly references exist for scholars to draw on to understand the state-of-the art of the study of life-span development. However, such resources have not existed before the present Handbook . Because of this absence, there was no single reference work that developmental scientists or their students could consult to find a thorough, integrative presentation of the breadth of scholarship documenting the use of the relational, developmental systems ideas that frame the life-span study of human development. There was no single high-level reference that provided discussions of the usefulness of relational concepts in integrating and extending the range of substantive areas involved in studying development across the life span.

    Instead, to date, the key reference works available to developmental scientists and their students about the nature and scope of life-span change processes have been—paradoxically—age-segmented resources (e.g., Damon & Lerner, 2006; Lerner & Steinberg, 2009). In short, despite the important and rich theoretical and substantive work that is framed by perspectives on human development that encompass the life span, there has been no single reference work that presents the top-tier developmental science work pertinent to such processes.

    The goal of this Handbook is to provide such a scholarly resource. It is the first-ever reference work to present—through the top-tier scholars in developmental science—the accumulated knowledge about the description, explanation, and implications for optimizing applications (i.e., applications that have the potential to maximize the chances for positive human development) of development across the life span. My fellow editors and I have the aspiration that this Handbook will constitute a watershed event in the development of life-span developmental scholarship. With the publication of this Handbook, we believe a compelling scholarly alternative will exist to counter both split depictions of developmental processes (e.g., studying childhood and adulthood as if they were composed of distinct, completely discontinuous processes) and split explanations of the changes that occur across the life span.

    We hope that this first edition of the Handbook will serve as a touchstone for current and future researchers and instructors. As such, future editions of the Handbook may provide an even richer depiction of the course of development across the breadth of the life span than is possible in this edition. Given that developmental science has had for so long an age-specific focus, several topics discussed across the two volumes of this Handbook remain underexplored, perhaps particularly with respect to the later adult years. When the literature was not available to discuss a particular topic in depth with regard to a portion of the life span, the authors point to this situation and suggest ways to expand the topic more fully across the life span. We believe that their ideas for future scholarship are persuasive—indeed, compelling. As such, our ultimate hope is that future editions of the Handbook will reflect the continued theoretical, methodological, and empirical refinement of the concepts about development that began to coalesce in the hills of West Virginia more than four decades ago.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    There are numerous people to whom the editors of this Handbook owe enormous thanks for their contributions. Clearly, we are deeply grateful to the colleagues who contributed to this work, both for their superb scholarly contributions and for their commitment to working collaboratively to produce this Handbook. We also appreciate greatly the unflagging support of our superb editor at John Wiley & Sons, Patricia Rossi. Her commitment to the vision of this Handbook and her support for the quality of contribution we sought to make to developmental science were essential, indeed invaluable, assets throughout our work. We are also grateful to Leslie Dickinson and Jarrett M. Lerner, successive Managing Editors at the Institute for Applied Research at Tufts University, for their superb editorial work. Their commitment to quality and productivity, and their resiliency in the face of the tribulations of manuscript production, are greatly admired and deeply appreciated. I am also grateful to the National 4-H Council, the Philip Morris Smoking Prevention Department, the John Templeton Foundation, the Thrive Foundation for Youth, and the National Science Foundation for supporting my work during the development of this project.

    Finally, my co-editors and I dedicate this Handbook to Paul B. Baltes, one of the pillars of 20th century developmental science and, across the last third of the 20th century, the key intellectual and professional force involved in establishing and enabling the flourishing of theory and research about life-span development. His intellect, leadership, generosity, kindness, and wisdom are warmly remembered and sorely missed.

    Richard M. Lerner

    Medford, MA

    October 1, 2009

    REFERENCES

    Baltes, P. B. (Ed.). (1978). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.

    Baltes, P. B. (1979). On the potential and limits of child development: Life-span developmental perspectives. Newsletter of the Society of Research in Child Development, 1-4.

    Baltes, P. B. (1983). Life-span developmental psychology: Observations on history and theory revisited. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Historical and philosophical perspectives (pp. 79-111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611-626.

    Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1979). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.

    Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1980). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 3). New York: Academic Press.

    Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1982). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 4). New York: Academic Press.

    Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1983). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5). New York: Academic Press.

    Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. (Eds.). (1984). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press.

    Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1986). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 7). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1988a). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 8). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1988b). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 9). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1990). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 10). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Baltes, P. B., Freund, A. M., & Li, S.-C. (2005). The psychological science of human aging. In M. Johnson, V. L. Bengston, P. G. Coleman, & T. B. L. Kirkwood (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of age and ageing (pp. 47-71). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Lifespan theory in developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.) Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 569-664). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1977). Life-span developmental psychology: Introduction to research methods. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Baltes, P. B., & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.). (1973). Life-span developmental psychology: Personality and socialization. New York: Academic Press.

    Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 81-95.

    Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Bornstein, M. H. (1995). Parenting infants. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 3-39). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.). (2005). Developmental science: An advanced textbook (5th ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.). (2010). Developmental science: An advanced textbook (6th ed.). New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.

    Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 807-863). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: Wiley.

    Brandtstädter, J. (2006). Action perspectives on human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 516-568). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Brim, O. G., & Kagan, J. (Eds.). (1980). Constancy and change in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 793-828). Editor in chief: W. Damon. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (2006). The making of developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 89-165). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Editors-in-Chief). (2006). Handbook of child psychology (6th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Datan, N., & Reese, H. W. (Eds.). (1977). Life-span developmental psychology: Dialectical perspectives on experimental research. New York: Academic Press.

    Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998). The life course and human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 939-991). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: Wiley.

    Elder, G. H., Modell, J., & Parke, R. D. (Eds.). (1993). Children in time and place: Developmental and historical insights. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Emmerich, W. (1968). Personality development and concepts of structure. Child Development, 39, 671-690.

    Featherman, D. L., Lerner, R. M., & Perlmutter, M. (Eds.). (1992). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 11). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Featherman, D. L., Lerner, R. M., & Perlmutter, M. (Eds.). (1994). Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 12). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory: An integrative approach. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Freund, A. M. (2007). Differentiating and integrating levels of goal representation: A life-span perspective. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, J. E. Nurmi, & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Personal project pursuit: Goals, action and human flourishing (pp. 247-270). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration of selection, optimization, and compensation: An action-theoretical conceptualization of a theory of developmental regulation. In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), Control of human behaviour, mental processes and consciousness (pp. 35-58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Freund, A. M., Li, K. Z. H., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). The role of selection, optimization, and compensation in successful aging. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action and development: Origins and functions of intentional self-development (pp. 401-434). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Freund, A. M., Nikitin, J., & Ritter, J. O. (2009). Psychological consequences of longevity: The increasing importance of self-regulation in old age. Human Development, 52, 1- 37.

    Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing nature-nurture: Prenatal roots of instinctive behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (2006). The significance of biology for human

    development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 210-257). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Goulet, L. R., & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.). (1970). Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Greenberg, G., & Tobach, E. (Eds.) Behavioral evolution and integrative levels. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Hall, G. S. (1922). Senescence: The last half of life. New York: Appleton.

    Havighurst, R. J. (1951). Developmental tasks and education. New York: Longmans.

    Hess, E. H. (1970). Ethology and developmental psychology. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 1-38). New York: Wiley.

    Kuo, Z.-Y. (1976). The dynamics of behavior development: An epigenetic view. New York: Plenum.

    Lamb, M. E. (1977). A reexamination of the infant social world. Human Development, 20, 65-85.

    Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. P., & Charnov, E. L. (1985). Infant-mother attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Lerner, R. M. (2006). Developmental science, developmental systems, and contemporary theories of human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 1-17). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Lerner, R. M., & Overton, W. F. (2008). Exemplifying the integrations of the relational developmental system: Synthesizing theory, research, and application to promote positive development and social justice. Journal of Adolescent Research, 23, 245-255.

    Lerner, R. M., & Steinberg, L. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Lewis, M. (1972). State as an infant-environment interaction: An analysis of mother infant behavior as a function of sex. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 95-121.

    Lewis, M., & Lee-Painter, S. (1974). An interactional approach to the mother-infant dyad. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregivers (pp. 21-48). New York: Wiley

    Lewis, M., & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.). (1974). The effect of the infant on its caregivers. New York: Wiley.

    Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (1998). Person-context interaction theories. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 685-759). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: Wiley.

    Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (2006). The person in context: A holistic- interactionistic approach. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 400-464). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    McClearn, G. R. (1970). Genetic influences on behavior and development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 39-76). New York: Wiley.

    Mussen, P. H. (Ed.). (1970). Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Nesselroade, J. R., & Reese, H. W. (Eds.). (1973). Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Nikitin, J., & Freund, A. M. (2008). Hoping to be liked or wishing not to be rejected: Conflict and congruence of social approach and avoidance motivation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 57, 90-111.

    Novikoff, A. B. (1945a). The concept of integrative levels and biology. Science, 101, 209-215.

    Novikoff, A. B. (1945b). Continuity and discontinuity in evolution. Science, 101, 405-406.

    Overton, W. F. (1973). On the assumptive base of the nature-nurture controversy: Additive versus interactive conceptions. Human Development, 16, 74-89.

    Overton, W. F. (1975). General systems, structure and development. In K. Riegel & G. Rosenwald (Eds.), Structure and transformation: Developmental aspects (pp. 61-81). New York: Wiley Interscience.

    Overton, W. F. (1978). Klaus Riegel: Theoretical contribution to concepts of stability and change. Human Development, 21, 360-363.

    Overton, W. F. (2003). Development across the life span: Philosophy, concepts, theory. In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6. Developmental psychology (pp. 13-42). Editor in chief: B. Weiner. New York: Wiley.

    Overton, W. F. (2006). Developmental psychology: Philosophy, concepts, methodology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.). Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development. Volume 1 of (6th ed.) (pp. 18-88). Editors-in-chief: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Overton, W., & Reese, H. (1981). Conceptual prerequisites for an understanding of stability-change and continuity-discontinuity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 99-123.

    Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and theories of development. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory (pp. 115-145). New York: Academic.

    Riegel, K. F. (1975). Toward a dialectical theory of human development. Human Development, 18, 50-64.

    Riegel, K. F. (1976). The dialectics of human development. American Psychologist, 31, 689-700.

    Riley, M. W. (Ed.). (1979). Aging from birth to death. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.

    Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models in early social relations. Human Development, 18, 65-79.

    Schneirla, T. C. (1957). The concept of development in comparative psychology. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development: An issue in the study of human behavior (pp. 78-108). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Stevenson, H. W. (1970). Learning in children. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 849-938). New York: Wiley.

    Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 563-633). Editor in chief: W. Damon. New York: Wiley.

    Thompson, R. A., & Lamb, M. E. (1986). Infant-mother attachment: New directions for theory and research. In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 7, pp. 1-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Tobach, E., & Greenberg, G. (1984). The significance of T. C. Schneirla’s contribution to the concept of levels of integration. In G. Greenberg & E. Tobach (Eds.), Behavioral evolution and integrative levels (pp. 1-8). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    von Bertalanffy, L. (1933). Modern theories of development. London: Oxford University Press.

    von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory. New York: Braziller.

    White, S. H. (1970). The learning theory tradition and child psychology. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 657-701). New York: Wiley.

    Wohlwill, J. F. (1973). The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press.

    Contributors

    Carolyn M. Aldwin

    Oregon State University

    Corvallis, Oregon

    Toni C. Antonucci

    University of Michigan

    Ann Arbor, Michigan

    Kenneth E. Barron

    James Madison University

    Harrisonburg, Virginia

    John E. Bates

    Indiana University

    Bloomington, Indiana

    Kira Birditt

    University of Michigan

    Ann Arbor, Michigan

    Daria K. Boeninger

    Arizona State University

    Tempe, Arizona

    Catherine E. Bowen

    Jacobs University

    Bremen, Germany

    Molly R. Butterworth

    University of Utah

    Salt Lake City, Utah

    C. Sue Carter

    University of Illinois at Chicago

    Chicago, Illinois

    Dante Cicchetti

    University of Minnesota

    Minneapolis/St. Paul, Minnesota

    John Colombo

    University of Kansas

    Lawrence, Kansas

    David E. Conroy

    The Pennsylvania State University

    University Park, Pennsylvania

    Keith S. Cox

    Northwestern University

    Evanston, Illinois

    Lisa M. Diamond

    University of Utah

    Salt Lake City, Utah

    Andrew J. Elliot

    Rochester University

    Rochester, New York

    Christopher P. Fagundes

    University of Utah

    Salt Lake City, Utah

    Katherine L. Fiori

    Adelphi University

    Garden City, New York

    Constance A. Flanagan

    The Pennsylvania State University

    University Park, Pennsylvania

    Alexandra M. Freund

    University of Zurich

    Zurich, Switzerland

    G. John Geldhof

    University of Kansas

    Lawrence, Kansas

    Jackson A. Goodnight

    Indiana University

    Bloomington, Indiana

    Daniel Grühn

    North Carolina State University

    Raleigh, North Carolina

    Daniel Hart

    Rutgers university

    Camden, New Jersey

    Melissa Hines

    University of Cambridge

    Cambridge, England

    Lisa M. H. Jackey

    University of Michigan

    Ann Arbor, Michigan

    Gisela Labouvie-Vief

    University of Geneva

    Geneva, Switzerland

    Michael E. Lamb

    University of Cambridge

    Cambridge, England

    Peter Levine

    Tufts University

    Medford, Massachusetts

    Marc D. Lewis

    University of Toronto

    Toronto, Ontario

    Todd D. Little

    University of Kansas

    Lawrence, Kansas

    Dan P. McAdams

    Northwestern University

    Evanston, Illinois

    Kou Murayama

    Tokyo Institute of Technology

    Tokyo, Japan

    Stephen W. Porges

    University of Illinois at Chicago

    Chicago, Illinois

    W. George Scarlett

    Tufts University

    Medford, Massachusetts

    Alice C. Schermerhorn

    Indiana University

    Bloomington, Indiana

    Ursula M. Staudinger

    Jacobs University

    Bremen, Germany

    Joseph Studer

    University of Geneva

    Geneva, Switzerland

    Rebecca Todd

    University of Toronto

    Toronto, Ontario

    Amy Eva Alberts Warren

    Tufts University

    Medford, Massachusetts

    Xiaowen Xu

    University of Toronto

    Toronto, Ontario

    Loriena A. Yancura

    University of Hawaii, Manoa

    Honolulu, Hawaii

    James Youniss

    Catholic University

    Washington, D.C.

    Jonathan F. Zaff

    Tufts University

    Medford, Massachusetts

    CHAPTER 1

    Introduction

    Social and Emotional Development across the Life Span

    ALEXANDRA M. FREUND and MICHAEL E. LAMB

    There are numerous handbooks on various aspects of psychology, covering topics as diverse as emotion, motivation, or social psychology. Handbooks of psychology typically include a volume on development, and there also exist two age-defined handbooks about development, the Handbook of Child Psychology, the sixth edition of which was edited by William Damon and Richard M. Lerner (2006), and the Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, the third edition of which was edited by Richard M. Lerner and Laurence Steinberg (2009). Although some of the finest chapters on life-span development were published in these handbooks, there was no high-level reference work reviewing theory and data about all aspects of psychology across the entire life span until the publication of the present Handbook. Our efforts to commission and edit the current volume were predicated on the assumption that a complete and satisfying understanding of any area of functioning requires adoption of a life-span perspective.

    Most research and theory in developmental psychology is still clearly concerned with specific phases of the life span. This specialization by researchers has a pragmatic rationale; to work across the full age spectrum would require so much technical and methodological expertise and an understanding of so many different, topic-specific paradigms that one laboratory would not be capable of mastering them all. In addition, the training we provide students of developmental science militates against the acquisition and development of such integrative, life-span knowledge even today. Lecture classes or seminars typically deal with one phase of development, such as emotional development in childhood or self and identity in adolescence. This focus implies that separate age phases constitute meaningful units for studying development. Moreover, most students (especially at the postgraduate level) are likely to study either child development or adult development and aging, giving them expertise regarding one phase of development but not of the entire life span. With this Handbook of Life-Span Development, we hope to begin breaking that pattern by commissioning thorough overviews of development in various functional domains across the entire life span.

    When conceptualizing this Handbook, we first took seriously the idea that development is a lifelong phenomenon. To the extent permitted by the knowledge base within each substantive area of focus included in this volume, the contributors have documented how the processes of development—despite qualitative or quantitative discontinuities (as well, of course, as continuities in these dimensions)—begin in utero and extend across the entire ontogenetic span, ending only with death. Unfortunately, however, variations in the extent to which this orientation to life-span developmental processes has come to characterize the study of different aspects of social and emotional development ensure that there are differences in the degree to which the areas of research reviewed in this volume have been studied across the life span.

    As one might expect in a field that tends to describe and study development in such discrete age-graded phases as infancy, childhood, adolescence, adulthood, or old age, our requests of the contributors were daunting and demanding. Most had expertise regarding one or two phases, rather than the entire life span. Although readers will be impressed by how well the contributors have mastered the tasks assigned to them, they will also notice a tendency for many chapters to include more detailed analyses of some phases of life than others. To the extent that these differences in amount of detail reflect differences in the amount of research that has been conducted, we hope that the evident gaps will prompt students and scholars of development to embrace perspectives that encompass the entire life span in their future research and conceptualization.

    Second, we have tried in this volume to cover all the diverse topics that comprise the field of social and emotional development. Of course, domains of development can be partitioned in many ways. In the end, the choices we made were largely dictated by the extent to which different topics had been conceptualized and studied, and the chapters included in the volume include different levels of analysis, ranging from the molecular focus on peptides, genes, hormones, and the neurological bases of development, to the macro-level analysis of such constructs as the development of self-efficacy and developmental psychopathology.

    Third, when charting the terrain of emotional and social development across the life span, we also sought to cover different experiential or functional aspects of development, including emotions, motivation, and temperament, as well as social and intimate relationships. We do not want to imply that these functional domains are separate; they are not. Humans experience emotions in social contexts, and these experiences are influenced by their motivations as well as their temperaments, for example. However, the focus on different functional domains makes a project like this Handbook feasible, while helping to create a holistic understanding of development.

    AN OVERVIEW OF THIS VOLUME

    Our vision for this volume is instantiated in the chapters that it comprises. The chapters are organized into several areas of scholarship pertinent to social and emotional development.

    Emotional Regulation across the Life Span

    In the second and third chapters in this volume, Porges and Carter (Chapter 2) and Lewis, Todd, and Xu (Chapter 3) place considerable emphasis on the neurobiological factors that influence or control individual behavior across the life span.

    Porges and Carter focus on the neurobiological factors that make social bonding and social interaction possible across the life span. The phylogenetic perspective they introduce draws extensively on research conducted using social animals, including voles, as well as research on individuals whose intrinsic characteristics make it easier to understand the underlying processes, either because they are especially young and immature or because they have conditions such as autistic spectrum disorders that compromise their capacities. Porges and Carter proceed to document the role of neuropeptides in promoting the development of social relationships, in part by inhibiting tendencies to reject or aggress against strangers (including offspring) and also explain how and why the brain, using the vagus nerve, modulates psychological reactions to different social stimulus. Interestingly, as they show, the same basic processes mediate interactions between individuals and their environments, especially but not exclusively their social environments, across the life span. Evidence regarding the ability of this model to explain key aspects of social behavior in childhood should, these authors hope, prompt more extensive research on the interface between neurobiological processes and social behavior at other stages of the life span.

    Lewis, Todd, and Xu’s approach to emotional regulation, as presented in Chapter 3, also focuses on fundamental processes, including those that determine both the extent and nature of emotional arousal. Lewis and colleagues make clear that the underlying neural mechanisms tend to be quite immature at birth. Furthermore, although developmental change is rapid in the early years, changes continue to take place more or less across the life span, with mature or optimal function emerging only in early adulthood and with neurological changes underlying declines in behavioral function in later adulthood and old age. Thus, the capacity to regulate emotions, especially in response to challenges posed by intrinsic (e.g., pain, illness) and extrinsic (e.g., fear-provoking stimuli or separation from a source of comfort) factors, develops gradually, with significant individual differences attributable to variation in the behavior of others (especially parents and care providers) during the phase of life when regulatory capacity and control shift from extrinsic to intrinsic control.

    As with Lewis and his colleague’s examination of emotional regulation, Labouvie-Vief, Grühn, and Studer’s analysis of the dynamic integration of emotion and cognition (Chapter 4) adopts a dynamic systems approach to the understanding of emotional development. Labouvie-Vief and colleagues present a model of emotional development inspired by Piaget’s seminal work on child development in two ways: by acknowledging the importance of striving for equilibrium in developing systems and by taking seriously the intricate interplay and connectedness of emotion and cognition. Their model seeks to explain how people develop by navigating between the needs for safety and well-being on one hand and the need for increased mastery and control on the other. According to their model, the opposing

    Enjoying the preview?
    Page 1 of 1