Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

Only $11.99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

Fish Pheromones and Related Cues
Fish Pheromones and Related Cues
Fish Pheromones and Related Cues
Ebook813 pages8 hours

Fish Pheromones and Related Cues

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars

()

Read preview

About this ebook

Pheromones are chemical cues that pass between members of the same species that convey specific, adaptive information.  These cues, and related chemicals whose function are less well defined, are especially important to fishes because of their aquatic environments and complex behaviors.   Pheromones are also of increasing interest in both basic and applied aspects of fish biology because they can be used to manipulate fish behavior and may explain phenomena such as fishery collapse.  Fish Pheromones and Related Cues provides a timely synthesis of this growing body of research in freshwater and marine species and explores everything from how these chemical signals evolved, are produced, released and then processed, and finally to potential applications in fish culture and conservation.  

 

Fish Pheromones and Related Cues opens with a useful overview on the theory of chemical communication and definitions. Chapters then progress by examining the biological importance of pheromones in inter- and intra-species communication, the role these chemical cues play in a variety biological functions from reproduction to predation, and then how they evolved and are detected and recognized by fish nervous systems. Final chapters provide valuable insight into how pheromones can be measured, how pheromonal disruption can explain effects of environmental pollution, and lastly how they pheromones are being applied in real-world efforts to culture fish species and to conserve our wild populations and control invasive species.

 

 With far-reaching economic, evolutionary and ecological implications, Fish Pheromones and Related Cues will be an essential volume for anyone working in the fields of chemical communication, fish biology, fisheries science, aquatic conservation, ecology, invasive species control, and aquaculture
LanguageEnglish
PublisherWiley
Release dateDec 16, 2014
ISBN9781118794715
Fish Pheromones and Related Cues

Related to Fish Pheromones and Related Cues

Related ebooks

Agriculture For You

View More

Related articles

Related categories

Reviews for Fish Pheromones and Related Cues

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars
0 ratings

0 ratings0 reviews

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

    Book preview

    Fish Pheromones and Related Cues - P. W. Sorensen

    Preface

    It has been known for at least a century that fish, the largest and most diverse group of vertebrates, are strongly influenced by chemicals that they themselves release into the water. Some of these stimuli seem to be discerned by innate mechanisms by all members of their species and are called pheromones. Other types of related organismal chemical cues appear to have less prescribed actions and may at times be learned. Together, these cues play profound and varied roles in the lives of fish, ranging from avoiding danger to synchronizing reproduction. This book, the first on fish pheromones and related cues, reviews all aspects of conspecific chemical cues in fish. It adopts a broad, systems-level approach to encourage new integrative thinking.

    The first chapter was written by the primary editor to define terms and provide common language for the entire book. A second set of chapters reviews how conspecific chemical cues are used by fishes. Both pheromones and related conspecific cues that may be learned are addressed because the distinction can be vague. A third set of chapters addresses mechanisms of pheromone detection and production as well our detection mechanisms. Lastly, applications in fisheries management and culture are addressed. An afterword summarizes some key points and future directions.

    A wide range of authors have generously contributed to this book which I hope will invigorate the field and stimulate another book in the near future.

    Peter W. Sorensen

    Chapter 1

    Introduction to Pheromones and Related Chemical Cues in Fishes

    Peter W. Sorensen

    University of Minnesota, St. Paul, USA

    1.1 CHEMICAL INFORMATION TRANSFER IN FISH

    Information transfer between fishes, the largest and most diverse group of vertebrates, has long been both of practical importance and a source of wonder given the evolutionary, ecological, and economic importance of this group. Chemicals play an especially significant role in this process, presumably because they function well in vast dark spaces, can encode a great deal of information, are readily soluble, and are inherently honest. This book specifically addresses how and why chemical information is transferred between fishes of the same species. First I define some basic terminology to promote clarity and then I introduce some other terms, types of chemical cues, and principals along with the chapters which discuss them. Information transfer between different species is not explicitly addressed except how it might occur as part of transfer within a species. This section introduces these terms and issues, and then where more information on them can be found in the book.

    1.2 TERMINOLOGY

    1.2.1 Overview

    Because the terminology used to describe conspecific cues has been used in many ways since the term pheromone was first coined 50 years ago (Karlson and Luscher, 1959), I suggest and define some terms in this introductory section to provide clarity. Definitions were chosen for practical reasons and to be consistent with those used by researchers outside the world of fishes. Emphasis is placed on recent work by Tristram Wyatt (2003, 2010). Authors were asked to consider the terminology suggested herein, but not necessarily to use it if their opinions differed. Information transfer between members of different species (kariomones) is not directly addressed in this book.

    1.2.2 Pheromones

    Following Tristram Wyatt (2010), a pheromone may be defined as: molecules that are evolved cues which elicit a specific reaction, for example a stereotyped behavior and/or development process in a conspecific. This definition is closely based on the original definition by Karlson and Luscher (1959). Key elements are that pheromones are evolved cues (stimuli whose production is in some way adaptive), species-wide (used by all members of the species), and that some type of innate recognition is implied. Nevertheless, responses to pheromones can be conditional on context, and/or internal state. Similarly, although pheromone composition (pheromones may be single compounds or mixtures) may vary slightly among individuals, this variation is expected to reflect physiological state (e.g., dominance) and not individual eccentricities or identity. In other words, I believe that the information contained in pheromones should be equally relevant to all individuals of the species (e.g. ‘shared’ by all members of that species). This type of rigidity appears to be associated with some type of specialized neural mechanisms. In contrast, organismal chemical cues that vary between individuals of the same species (either because of their chemical structures and/or because they are learned) should not considered to be pheromones but ‘related conspecific cues.’

    Identification of pheromones in fish is difficult and requires isolation of the released chemical(s) and proof that it/they elicit a specific adaptive response via some type of innate (neural) mechanism. This is not to say that learning cannot be involved but it should presumably be highly prescribed so that all members of the species recognize the same cues. Indeed, only a handful of fish pheromones meet these criteria these species include goldfish (Carassius auratus) hormonal sex pheromones, sea lamprey (Petromyzon marinus) migratory and sex pheromones, the Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) sex pheromone, and perhaps the reproductive cues used by the African catfish (Clarias gariepinus). Nevertheless, there is a great deal of circumstantial evidence that pheromones are commonly produced and used by most, if not all, fishes. Their functions are diverse and include conspecific recognition, recognition of reproductive state, and the presence of injured conspecifics. These functions will be reviewed in Section 1.3; but first, a few terms that are commonly used to describe the pheromones in fish and other species are defined.

    Primer Pheromone

    A priming pheromone is a conspecific chemical cue that drives an adaptive developmental or otherwise wholly physiological response in an exposed conspecific. All members of a species in the same physiological state should typically be similarly affected. Examples include hormonal cues that drive endocrine changes in exposed conspecifics and alarm cues that change growth characteristics of exposed conspecifics.

    Releaser Pheromone

    A releasing pheromone is a conspecific chemical cue that drives a rapid, adaptive, and innate behavioral response in a conspecific. All members of a species in the same physiological state should typically be similarly affected. Examples include hormonal metabolites that drive sexual arousal. Pheromones may have both releasing and priming effects.

    Pheromone Mixture

    Pheromones may comprise mixtures of chemical cues that may act on their own or synergistically. A blend is very specific type of mixture that requires that multiple components be present in very specific ratios for the mixture to have activity. Although commonly described in insects, no examples appear to exist in fish. By contrast, a complex is mixture of pheromonal components that can assume different functions depending on mixture composition. Ratios are not necessarily of primary importance but the overall composition is. Recent studies suggest that complexes in goldfish (see chapter 2) may include nonhormonal components that encode species identity and hormonal components that encode sexual condition.

    1.2.3 Signature Mixtures

    Fish, like all other vertebrates, release and learn to recognize conspecific chemicals for various purposes such as individual and kin recognition. Following Wyatt (2010), the term signature mixture is used for non-phermonal but related cues that can be defined as variable chemical mixtures which are released by organisms and learned by other conspecifics to recognize individual or a member of some type of social group. The term signature implies some level of individuality; unlike pheromones, these cues are not anonymous and they are learned. The manner in which they are learned need not be prescribed. Further, their composition is typically complex and variable. In fishes, as in mammals, these cues appear to be commonly used to mediate recognition of individuals within social hierarchies or perhaps other aspect of special value such that they have been subject to recent stress. In mammals, these cues often appear to be genetically based on the major histocompatability complex (MHC) that codes genetic identity, but this possibility has not been fully resolved in fishes. A signature mixture is expected to be highly context dependent and may change with diet and other environmental factors. Recognition appears to require combinatorial responses of broad elements of the olfactory system. Signature mixtures may be found together with pheromones.

    Identification of a signature mixture requires isolation of the released chemical(s) and proof that it elicits an adaptive response that is learned. Several examples have been described in fish. For example, Bryant and Atema (1987) show that diet influences production of odors associated with social hierarchy in the bullhead catfish, Ictalurus nebulosus, and that amino acids change in urine. Fish have also been show to readily learn to recognize the odors of conspecifics that have been attacked (Chivers and Smith, 1998). These functions will be reviewed in Section 1.3. Many ornamental odors such as those reviewed by Lynda Corkum and Karen Cogliati in Chapter 4 may fit this definition.

    1.2.4 Other Definitions Relevant to this Book

    A few other definitions associated with production and detection of conspecific chemical cues and signals are defined to promote clarity. Some of these definitions have also been the subject of considerable controversy which is not discussed here as they are defined largely for operational reasons.

    Cues refer to any stimulus that elicits a sensory response in an animal’s sensory system.

    Signals are a prescribed set of cue(s) whose chemical identity has been influenced by evolutionary processes and may thus be considered to be specialized.

    Communication may be defined as the exchange of adaptive information (e.g. signals) between two conspecifics.

    The olfactory sense is the chemosensory component of the cranial nerve 1 (i.e., taste and common chemical sense are not included). It is also known as the sense of smell.

    An odorant is a molecule that binds with olfactory receptor(s) and stimulates the olfactory sense.

    An odor is an identifiable suite of odorant(s) that an animal’s olfactory system can discriminate.

    Fish are chordates with gills and fins that spend most or all of their live in water. (This book will address jawless, cartilaginous, and boney fishes.)

    1.3 FUNCTIONS SERVED BY PHEROMONES AND RELATED CUES

    1.3.1 Overview

    Pheromones and related conspecific cues are known and defined by their biological function(s). Although these functions are diverse, they can be placed into five broad categories as outlined below. Some of these categories are not mutually exclusive, and presumably others may still await discovery.

    1.3.2 Alarm Cues

    As with other organisms, fish have evolved to recognize and respond to stimuli associated with the risk of predation, of which chemicals released by injured conspecifics are one (Chivers and Smith, 1998; Brown, 2003; Wisenden, Volbrecht, and Brown, 2004; Ferrari et al., 2010). Dozens of examples exist of fish-fleeing areas that contain extracts of damaged conspecific skin odor or reacting in other adaptive manners. In many instances, these responses seem to be species-specific, but this is not always the case. Also, there is evidence that fish can learn to respond to other species if they are damaged. These can be complex multicomponent cues, and there is even evidence that some can serve as primers. For example, the crucian carp, C. carassius, becomes more deep-bodied when exposed to damaged conspecific skin (Bronmark and Miner, 1992). Laboratory behavior studies suggest that hypoxanthine-3-N-oxide plays a role in the alarm response (Brown et al., 2000), but this compound is yet to be measured in the water or shown to be detected by the fish nervous system. Quite possibly, multiple cue types are involved. Brian Wisenden reviews alarm responses in a critical manner while evaluating specific evidence of innate versus learned recognition in Chapter 6.

    1.3.3 Nonreproductive Recognition and Aggregation

    Conspecific recognition is important to fishes, and chemical cues appear to play a significant role in this process, especially amongst fishes that live in dark and/or deeper waters (Hemmings, 1966; Sisler and Sorensen, 2008). One important function is to promote shoaling and aggregation among nonreproductive individuals that seek to find each other to either avoid predation or locate food. Another function is to facilitate migratory orientation by adults or juveniles that seek habitat populated by conspecifics. Freshwater eels (Anguilla sp.), chars (Salvelinus sp.), galaxids (Galaxias sp.), and lampreys (Petromyzontidae) use conspecific body odors in this manner (Baker and Montgomery, 2001). It also appears that these conspecific cues may often be mixtures of nonhormonal body metabolites that function together with hormonal pheromones as part of pheromone complexes (Sorensen, Scott, and Kihslinger, 2000; Levesque et al., 2011; Lim and Sorensen, 2011). Both bile acids (Selset and Døving, 1980) and l-amino acids (Saglio and Blanc, 1989) have been implicated in species recognition, but only for the sea lamprey have they been identified and then as sulfated bile sterols (Sorensen et al., 2005). This topic is reviewed by Peter Sorensen and Cindy Baker in Chapter 2.

    1.3.4 Individual and Kin Recognition

    As is the case with mammals, the complex social systems used by some fish have favored the evolution of chemosensory mechanisms to determine relatedness of conspecifics (Olsén et al., 1998). Functions of these familial odors include recognition of young and shoaling/schooling (Ward and Hart, 2003). In North American ictalurid catfish, at least some components of the odor used in individual recognition are l-amino acids (Bryant and Atema, 1987). Studies of salmonids suggest that kin odors are released in the urine and that a gene product associated with the major histocompatability complex (MHC) might be involved (Olsén, Grahn, and Lohm, 2002). The identity of individual, kin-specific odors is unknown although some speculate that peptide may be involved. This type of conspecific cue appears to represent a signature mixture and is reviewed by Ashley Ward in Chapter 5.

    1.3.5 Ornamental Odors

    Many species of fish are highly territorial and advertize their presence and identity using visual, acoustical, and chemical cues. Some fishes have specialized glands for the production of these cues (Bushmann, Burns, and Weitzman, 2002; Belanger, Corkum, and Zielinski, 2007), but the active components have not yet been identified. Both pheromones and signature mixtures can serve this function. Ornamental odors may assume communicatory roles. This topic is reviewed by Lynda Corkum and Karen Cogliati in Chapter 4.

    1.3.6 Reproductive Stimulants

    Arguably, the most important event in an organism’s life is finding a suitable mate and reproducing. Fish are no exception, and the challenges of life underwater appear to have favored the use of sexual signals including pheromones. A few of these have been identified, and the vast majority appears to be hormonal products and derivatives (hormonal pheromones) whose production presumably reflects inherent reproductive state and activity. Production, release, and response of select hormonal products have now been demonstrated in a few fishes: the goldfish, common carp (Cyprinus carpio), Atlantic salmon, and African catfish (Sorensen and Hoye, 2010). However, hundreds of species of fish from a broad variety of groups have now been shown to detect at least a few hormonal products with high sensitivity and specificity; therefore, the use of hormonal pheromones likely is widespread among fishes. Notable exceptions are a keto bile acid used by male sea lamprey (P. marinus; Li et al., 2002) and an unusual amino acid used by ovulated masu salmon (Oncorhynchus masou; Yambe et al., 2005). Hormonal pheromones have been especially well described among the minnows and carps where they function as changing mixtures in the contexts of other cues.

    Several functions have been elucidated for hormonal sex pheromones. First, there is evidence that at least a few species of fish recognize the gender of maturing conspecifics. For instance, male goldfish release the androgen androstenedione by which females recognize males (Sorensen, Pinillos, and Scott, 2005). In addition, various fishes use priming sex pheromones derived from prespawning hormones to predict spawning and respond with hormonal surges of their own. The best understood of these is 17α,20β-dihydroxy-4-pregnen-3-one that is released along with other conjugates by ovulated female carps detected at picomolar concentrations (Dulka et al., 1987). Hormonal pheromones also mediate mate recognition and sexual encounters between sexually active conspecific fishes. The F prostaglandins that serve to mediate ovulation have an especially prominent role in this process (Sorensen et al., 1988; Stacey and Sorensen, 2009). Interesting questions about hormonal sex pheromones are how they might have come into use, how pheromone identity might relate to reproductive mode, how they might encode species identity, and whether they may influence hormonal function. It is possible that hormonal cues function as part of complexes. This topic is reviewed by Norm Stacey in Chapter 3 in which he addresses some new work exploring evolutionary questions in the African cichlids.

    1.4 PHEROMONE IDENTITY, SYNTHESIS, AND RELEASE

    The few fish pheromones that have been definitely identified (i.e., isolated and measured in the water and then shown to elicit sensory and biological responses) are relatively simple unspecialized structures. No signature cues, with the possible exception of the relatively simple l-amino acid mixtures employed by Ictalurid catfish (Bryant and Atema, 1987, see above) have been identified. The use of such simple structures in conspecific signaling presumably reflects the origins of these cues as bodily metabolites. To date, F prostaglandins, various C18, C19, and C21 sex steroids, an amino acid, and bile acids have been shown to have pheromonal function in various fishes. Many of these structures are conjugated with sugars or sulfates, perhaps because they increase solubility. These structures were reviewed by Sorensen and Hoye (2010); therefore, they are not reviewed in this book.

    1.5 PHEROMONE DETECTION AND PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSIVENESS

    1.5.1 Overview

    Where studied, conspecific chemical cues in fishes have been found to be detected and discriminated by the olfactory system (cranial nerve 1). This also seems to be the case for all vertebrates and, presumably, reflects the inherent ability of this system to encode complex information and rapidly relay it to areas of the forebrain associated with social behaviors. Efforts to understand how social cues are processed in the fish olfactory system have focused on pheromones and the premise that they are discerned by specific components of the olfactory system. Nevertheless, a few studies suggest that this system also encodes signature information as it does in the mammals. Ongoing studies support this possibility, and they are reviewed herein. First, we address olfactory receptors (detection), then discrimination, and last responses (higher level function) and how these systems might be evolved.

    1.5.2 Pheromone Receptors

    The first step in the perception of a chemical cue involves binding of a ligand (odorant) to an olfactory receptor. There is every reason to believe that this is case with pheromones too, but it has yet to be directly demonstrated. Like other vertebrates, fish have many dozens of receptors of several types (Saraiva and Korsching, 2007). Pheromone receptors have unfortunately not been definitely isolated in fishes (although there is speculation (Bazáes, Olivares, and Schmachtenberg, 2013)); therefore, they are not reviewed.

    1.5.3 Olfactory Discrimination of Pheromones

    Following binding, electrical responses to odorants are transduced via the olfactory nerves whose activity creates neural maps of odor identity in the olfactory bulb. This is how a complex odor is discerned, and presumably pheromones have much simpler and more invariant maps than signature mixtures because only one receptor type is expressed in each olfactory receptor neuron. Various evidences, which include electrophysiological recordings, histological, and neural ablation, suggest that information on sex and alarm pheromones is conveyed by specific subclasses of olfactory neurons that project down the medial portions of the fish olfactory system (Hamdani and Døving, 2007). Although the crucian carp is perhaps the best understood model, there is compelling evidence that the olfactory systems of other fishes function in similar manners. Stine Lastein, El Hasan Hamdani, and Kjell Døving describe in Chapter 8 what we know about the key processes that underlie pheromone discrimination in fishes.

    1.5.4 Pheromonal Signaling and Communication

    Water-borne pheromones pass readily between conspecifics and present a myriad of opportunities to evolve and change with time. Thus, although many (most) pheromonal cues presumably evolved as unspecialized bodily metabolites whose detection instilled an advantage to the receiver, others with time have assumed secondary roles in which their production comes to impart an advantage to the donor. For example, male tilapia, Oreochromis mossambicus, maintain complex hierarchies and nests and have evolved urinary pheromones that convey their status to proximate conspecifics (Barata et al., 2007). This process involves various levels of physiological specialization that may involve specialization of cue production for its own sake and can be considered to be an example of true communication (Wisenden and Stacey, 2005; Stacey and Sorensen, 2009; Wyatt, 2010). Brian Wisenden examines how and why pheromonal cues may have come to be specialized in Chapter 7. New issues about definitions are also raised in the chapter.

    1.6 PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS OF FISH PHEROMONES

    1.6.1 Overview

    Chemical cues, and pheromones in particular, play critical roles in the lives of many fishes. Laboratory and field studies consistently find that fish that experience olfactory damage will often fail to find key habitat or mate. Similarly, other studies show that addition of small quantities of pheromones to the water can exert powerful, adaptive effects. The potency of pheromones and the ease with which they can be added to the water make them excellent candidates for managing fish in aquacultural settings or in the wild (invasive fish in particular). A key component of applying pheromones is to understand their distributions and concentrations. These topics are addressed herein.

    1.6.2 Effects of Pollution on the Perception of Conspecific Cues

    The olfactory system appears to be exclusively responsible for detection and processing of conspecific chemical cues; yet because olfactory receptors are freely exposed to the water, they are extremely susceptible to environmental damage. In addition, drugs and other water-borne contaminants may specifically disrupt neural function in this sensitive system. Sublethal effects of poor-water quality on chemical information transfer in fishes have been documented (Jaennson et al., 2007). Håkan Olsén reviews this fascinating and important topic along with the effects of pollutants on the olfactory sense in chapter 10.

    1.6.3 Application of Pheromones to the Management and Control of Wild Fisheries

    Fish pheromones have been shown to exert powerful influences on fish behavior and physiological function at subpicomolar concentrations. They are also easy to apply and, at least in theory, most are environmentally safe because of the specificity of their actions. Management of wild fish is currently challenged by difficulties of censusing fish or in the case of invasive fish, removing them. Fishery agencies are presently examining pheromones for control of the exploding problem of invasive fishes and fishery conservation. The sea lamprey control program has made significant contributions in understanding the biology and application of pheromones to this invasive in the Laurentian Great Lakes. Peter Sorensen examines some of these possibilities in chapter 12.

    1.6.4 Measuring and Interpreting Pheromones in the Water

    To use pheromones effectively, one needs to know how they are found in natural waters so that levels can be maintained. Two techniques have been developed: radioimmunoassay (Scott and Ellis, 2007) and mass-spectrometry (Fine and Sorensen, 2005). Michael Stewart and Peter Sorensen explore the potential of these techniques and what they have shown in Chapter 9.

    1.6.5 Applications of Pheromones in Marine Fish and Their Culture

    Pheromones are powerful modulators of fish reproductive behavior and physiology in both fresh and salt water, yet little is known about the latter. Pheromones have also been identified in several species that have commercial importance, some of which will not reproduce without endocrine treatment. Unlike hormones, pheromones can be applied to fish without handling—saving time, money, and stress. Peter Hubbard in Chapter 11 addresses whether and how pheromones are used by marine fishes and how they might be used in aquaculture while focusing mainly on marine species.

    1.7 SUMMARY

    Chemical information transfer between fishes of the same species can take many forms and exert powerful effects. These effects can be either innate or learned, and all appear to be mediated by the olfactory system that makes them susceptible to damage and manipulation. The complexity of these scenarios requires the use of many terms whose precise meaning should not be overinterpreted because in most conditions they represent continua rather than absolutes.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    Peter Sorensen thanks Brain Wisenden and many others including his students and postdocs and his former advisors for their help in two decades of research. Many granting agencies including The National Science Foundation, The National Institutes of Health, Sea Grant, The Minnesota Agricultural Experiment Station, The Great Lakes Fisheries Commission, and Minnesota Environment and Natural Resources Trust Fund have generously supported this research over what seems to be forever.

    REFERENCES

    Baker, C.F. and Montgomery, J.C. (2001) Species-specific attraction of migratory banded kokopu juveniles to adult pheromones. Journal of Biology, 58, 1221–1229.

    Bazáes, A., Olivares, J., and Schmachtenberg, O. (2013) Properties, projections and tuning of teleost olfactory receptor neurons. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 39, 451–464.

    Belanger, R.M., Corkum, L.D., and Zielinski, B.S. (2007) Differential behavioral responses by reproductive and non-reproductive male round gobies (Neogobius melanostomus) to the putative pheromone estrone. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology and Biochemistry: Part A Molecular and Integrative Physiology, 147, 77–83.

    Bronmark, C. and Miner, J.G. (1992) Predator-induced phenotypical change in body morphology in crucian carp.Science, 258, 1348–1350.

    Brown, G.E. (2003) Learning about danger: chemical alarm cues and local risk assessment in prey species. Fish and Fisheries, 4, 227–234.

    Brown, G.E., Adrian, J.C., Smyth, E. et al. (2000) Ostariophysan alarm pheromones: laboratory and field tests of the functional significance of nitric oxides. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 26, 154–163.

    Bryant, B. and Atema, J. (1987) Diet manipulation influences social behavior in catfish: importance of body odor. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 13, 1645–1666.

    Bushmann, P.J., Burns, J.R., and Weitzman, S.H. (2002) Gill-derived glands in glandulocaudine fishes (teleostei: Characidae: Glandulocaudinae). Journal of Morphology, 253, 187–195.

    Chivers, D.P. and Smith, R.J.F. (1998) Predator alarm system in aquatic predator-prey alarm systems. Ecoscience, 5, 338–352.

    Dulka, J.G., Stacey, N.E., Sorensen, P.W., and Van Der Kraak, G.J. (1987) A sex steroid pheromone synchronizes male-female spawning readiness in goldfish. Nature, 325, 251–253.

    Ferrari, C.O., Elvidge, C.K., Jackson, C.D. et al. (2010) The responses of prey fish to temporal variation in predation risk: sensory habituation or risk assessment? Behavioral Ecology, 21, 532–533.

    Fine, J.M. and Sorensen, P.W. (2005) Biologically-relevant concentrations of petromyzonol sulfate, a component of the sea lamprey migratory pheromone, measured in stream waters. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 31, 2205–2210.

    Hamdani, E.L. and Døving, K.B. (2007) The functional organization of the fish olfactory system. Progress in Neurobiology, 82, 80–86.

    Hemmings, C.C. (1966) Olfaction and vision in fish schooling. Journal of Experimental Biology, 45, 449–464.

    Jaennson, A., Scott, A.P., Moore, A. et al. (2007) Effects of a pyrethroid pesticide on endocrine responses to female odours and reproductive behaviour in male parr of brown trout (Salmo trutta L). Aquatic Toxicology, 81, 1–9.

    Karlson, P. and Luscher, M. (1959) Pheromones a new term for a class of biologically active substances. Nature, 183, 155–156.

    Levesque, H., Scaffidi, D., Polkinghorne, C.A., and Sorensen, P.W. (2011) A multi-component species identifying pheromone in the goldfish. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 37, 219–227.

    Li, W.M., Scott, A.P., Siefkes, M.J. et al. (2002) Bile acid secreted by a male sea lamprey that functions as a sex pheromone. Science, 296, 138–141.

    Lim, H.K. and Sorensen, P.W. (2011) Polar metabolites synergize the activity of prostaglandin F2α in a species-specific hormonal sex pheromone released by ovulated common carp. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 37, 695–704.

    Olsén, K.H., Grahn, M., Lohm, J., and Langefors, A. (1998) MHC and kin discrimination in juvenile Arctic char (Salvelinus alpinus L). Animal Behavior, 56, 319–327.

    Olsén, K.H., Grahn, M., and Lohm, J. (2002) Influence of MHC on kin discrimination in Arctic char (Salvelinus alpinus L). Journal of Chemical Ecology, 28, 793–795.

    Saglio, P.B. and Blanc, J.M. (1989) Intraspecific chemocommunication in immature goldfish, Carassius auratus L.: attraction in olfactometer to free amino acid fractions from skin extract. Biological Behavior, 14, 132–147.

    Saraiva, L.R. and Korsching, S.I. (2007) A novel olfactory receptor gene family in teleost fish. Genome Research, 17, 1448–1457.

    Scott, A.P. and Ellis, T. (2007) Measurement of fish steroids in the water—review. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 153, 392–400.

    Selset, R. and Døving, K.B. (1980) Behaviour of mature anadromous char (Salevenius alpinus L.) towards odorants of their own population. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 108, 113–121.

    Sisler, S.P. and Sorensen, P.W. (2008) Common carp and goldfish discern conspecific identity using chemical cues. Behaviour, 145, 1409–1425.

    Sorensen, P.W. and Hoye, T.H. (2010) Pheromones in vertebrates, in Chemical Ecology, Comprehensive Natural Products Chemistry II: Chemistry and Biology, vol. 4 (ed. K. Mori), Elsevier Press, Amsterdam/Heidelberg, pp. 225–262.

    Sorensen, P.W., Hara, T.J., Stacey, N.E., and Goetz, F.W. (1988) F prostaglandins function as potent olfactory stimulants that comprise the postovulatory female sex pheromone in goldfish. Biology of Reproduction, 39, 1039–1050.

    Sorensen, P.W., Scott, A.P., and Kihslinger, R.L. (2000) How common hormonal metabolites function as specific pheromones in the goldfish, in Proceedings of the Sixth International Symposium on the Reproductive Physiology of Fish (eds B. Norberg, O.S. Kjesbu, G.L. Taranger, E. Andersson, and S.O. Stefansson), John Grieg AS, Bergen, pp. 125–129.

    Sorensen, P.W., Fine, J.M., Dvornikovs, V. et al. (2005) Mixture of new sulfated steroids functions as a migratory pheromone in the sea lamprey. Nature Chemical Biology, 1, 324–328.

    Sorensen, P.W., Pinillos, M., and Scott, A.P. (2005) Sexually mature male goldfish release large quantities of androstenedione to the water where it functions as a pheromone. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 140, 164–175.

    Stacey, N.E. and Sorensen, P.W. (2009) Hormonal pheromones in fish, in Hormones, Brain and Behavior, 2nd edn (eds D.W. Pfaff, A.P. Arnold, A. Etgen, S. Fahrbach, and R. Rubin), Elsevier Press, San Diego, pp. 639–681.

    Ward, A.J.W. and Hart, P.J.B. (2003) The effects of kin and familiarity on interactions between fish. Fish and Fisheries, 4, 348–358.

    Wisenden, B.D. and Stacey, N.E. (2005) Fish semiochemicals and the evolution of communication networks, in Animal Communication Networks (ed. P. McGregor), Cambridge University Press, London, pp. 540–567.

    Wisenden, B.D., Volbrecht, K.A., and Brown, J.L. (2004) Is there a fish alarm cue? Affirming evidence from a wild study. Animal Behaviour, 67, 59–67.

    Wyatt, T.D. (2003) Pheromones and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Wyatt, T.D. (2010) Pheromones and signature mixtures: defining species-wide signals and variable cues for individuality in both invertebrates and vertebrates. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 196, 685–700.

    Yambe, H., Kitamura, S., Kamio, M. et al. (2005) L-Kynurenine, an amino acid identified as a sex pheromone in the urine of ovulated female masu salmon. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 103, 15370–15374.

    Chapter 2

    Species-Specific Pheromones and Their Roles in Shoaling, Migration, and Reproduction: A Critical Review and Synthesis

    Peter W. Sorensen1 and Cindy Baker2

    ¹ University of Minnesota, St. Paul, USA

    ² National Institute of Water and Atmospheric Research Ltd, Hamilton, New Zealand

    2.1 INTRODUCTION

    In the vast and dimly lit waters that characterize most aquatic ecosystems, fish need to find and recognize conspecifics for many reasons. Among these are the needs to find each other or shoal to find food efficiently while avoiding predators, to identify nursery habitat (which often contains conspecifics), and to find mates. All of these processes reflect a type of aggregation behavior, and thereby favor the use of conspecific odors. Such odors can travel great distances and convey large amounts of accurate information. Available evidence suggests that recognition of these odors is highly adaptive, common to all members of the species (species-wide), and based on highly prescribed processes that are either innate or involve some type of developmental process: these odors can be considered pheromones (Wyatt, 2010).

    This chapter evaluates what we know and what do not know about fish pheromones and how they serve species-wide and often species-specific functions. Species specificity is defining feature of these cues and a focus here yet it presents a paradox because all pheromonal compounds identified to date in fish are relatively common metabolites (Sorensen and Hoye, 2010). We focus on evaluating those experimental scenarios for which there is some type of biochemical as well as behavioral data that explicitly address species specificity - and they are surprisingly rare. First, we define and review the behavioral roles and chemistry of pheromones used in shoaling, then the use of conspecific odors in migration, and then their use in reproduction. Our focus is on describing and deciphering available high-quality data, allowing the data speak for itself. Last, we evaluate common properties of all of these types of aggregation pheromones while considering a unifying theory to explain commonalities and how the same cues may even be used by multiple life history stages and closely related species. The idea of pheromone complexes that are comprised of shifting but distinctive mixtures of compounds compounds is proposed.

    2.2 PHEROMONES AND NONREPRODUCTIVE SHOALING BEHAVIOR

    Very few fish live solitary lives; in addition to finding each other to mate, they often aggregate as juveniles to find food and/or habitat and to avoid predators (Pitcher and Parish, 1993). The process of aggregating requires that fish recognize each other; and while many types of sensory cues are involved in this process, odors (distinctive sets of chemical cues that are detected by the olfactory sense) appear to be the most important one. Shoals can be defined as loosely organized aggregations that help fish find food and habitats while avoiding predation (Pitcher and Parish, 1993). Shoals may at times contain other species, especially if these species have similar behaviors and ecological needs (Ward, 2015), and we will touch on that here. In additional, shoals may also contain sexually mature individuals; however, mating is typically not the primary reason for shoal formation. Interpretation of shoals with mature individuals is complicated by reproductive interactions; therefore, we do not discuss this scenario. Highly polarized and synchronized shoals are known as schools and appear to offer further advantages including increased swimming efficiency (Pitcher and Parish, 1993). We do not review schooling per se as it is seemingly not fundamentally different from shoaling. Shoaling is especially common among juvenile fish that find food and avoid predation through social interactions.

    The nature of shoaling varies with species and situation. While some fishes are very discerning about those they will shoal with (i.e., some select kin or members of the same population), others are less specific and will even shoal with congeners (Ward, Axford, and Krause, 2002; Ward, 2015). Not surprisingly, life history attributes appear to correlate with the specificity and nature of the odors used. Although possible relationships between kin, population, and species odors have not been studied, it seems likely that the former are subsets of the latter. In particular, data exist showing that fish can learn kin odor (Ward, 2015). How kin odor might translate to species-wide (species-specific) odors has seemingly not been studied, although it is interesting because it suggests flexibility in how conspecific identity may be discerned. Nevertheless, for the sake of simplicity, we focus on results of experimental studies that have directly tested the behavioral abilities of immature fishes to discern species identity independent of kinship in preference (head-to-head) tests using natural odors.

    While a vast ecological literature suggests that most fishes are attracted to conspecifics and form aggregations at some time in their lives (Liley, 1982), the actual specificity and mechanisms underlying this response has only been illuminated in a few species. While many studies have shown attraction to conspecific odors, only for a handful of species has some type of head-to-head preference test been conducted for assessing the specific ability of immature fishes to discriminate natural odors released by different species. Further, for only one species, the goldfish, Carassius auratus, has the chemical characteristics of the conspecific odor that they both respond to and discriminate (i.e. discern from other fish odors including the background) also been described; however, specific compounds have not been identified. In addition, no field tests of natural odors in a natural background have been conducted (Johnson et al., 2009), and our understanding of how immature fishes identify conspecifics using odor is extremely poor. Here, we review what little we know, which all suggests that odor-driven conspecific recognition is a common attribute of fishes and likely involves relatively complex mixtures.

    Conspecific recognition and discrimination using odor has been studied in five species of immature fishes. The roach, Rutilus rutilus, a European minnow, was the first species to have both the sensory basis of its shoaling behavior and its ability to discriminate examined (Hemmings, 1966). Maze studies demonstrated that it strongly prefers conspecific to heterospecific holding water. Time in the laboratory (i.e., experience) has no effect on this preference, suggesting that this behavior is not situation specific and likely innate (thus species-wide). A similar experiment with the Japanese catfish, Plotosus anguillaris (Kinosita, 1972), showed it too can discriminate and select conspecifics using simple body rinses. Similar responses to holding waters have since been noted in juvenile char, Salvelinus alpinus, for which amino acids shed via skin mucus were suggested to convey species information but unfortunately have never been tested (Hoglund and Astrand, 1973). Intriguingly, char are also known to recognize kin using, odor and this has been ascribed to learning (Olsen, Grahn, and Lohm, 2002). More recently, Ward, Axford, and Krause (2002) found that a European chub, Leuciscus cephalus, prefers to shoal with conspecifics over heterospecifics, and that chemical cues are more important than visual cues. The precise manner with which visual and odor cues complement each other has not yet been addressed nor has the possible role of experience (learning).

    A series of recent experiments using the goldfish and its close relative, the common carp (Cyprinus carpio), have clearly demonstrated that these species also recognize the odors of their conspecifics and that these odors have many chemical components. These studies used multiple stocks of fishes, suggesting the response is fundamental to these species and species-wide (Sorensen, unpublished data). Initial studies by Sisler and Sorensen (2008) discovered that sexually immature goldfish and common carp strongly prefer rinses of their own species versus those of six other species and that they continue to demonstrate these preferences in direct head-to-head tests (Fig. 2.1). Learning does not appear to have an apparent role in species recognition because cohousing goldfish with other species had no effect on their preferences, while olfactory ablation did block it. The chemical identity of conspecific odor recognized by immature goldfish was later characterized by Levesque et al. (2011) who fractionated immature goldfish holding waters and found activity in both the nonpolar and polar fractions, suggesting the pheromone comprises a mixture of compounds. Tests of conspecific bile acids (a class of odors suggested to have pheromonal function; Lastein, Hmadani, and Døving, 2015) suggested these are not important. Cross-tests of sexually mature males and females found all life stages are attracted to and attractive to all others: a common odor is released and recognized throughout the life cycle of this species. Levesque et al. (2011) conclude that goldfish employ a pheromone complex that contains polar and nonpolar components, the precise composition of which may change (by the addition of hormonal components) with maturational state while key species-specific elements remain constant.

    c2-fig-0001

    Figure 2.1. Preferences of immature goldfish for rinses of conspecifics and other fish species in a laboratory maze demonstrate the existence of a species-specific pheromone.

    Sisler, S.P. & Sorensen, P.W. Common carp and goldfish discern conspecific identity using chemical cues. Behaviour 145, 1409-1429. Copyright © Brill (2008).

    In summary, there is strong evidence that juveniles of many fishes use conspecific odors to mediate species recognition in shoaling and schooling behaviors. These odors seem to resemble kin and population odors; and although none have been identified, it seems that these may frequently be complex mixtures of simple, common metabolites. Further, these studies show that fish may release common suites of compounds throughout their lives that can be discerned by the olfactory sense, and thus lend them different meaning or even allow them to be part of other pheromones (i.e., it could provide the species-specific information that hormonal sex pheromonal cues seem to lack [see Section 2.4; Stacey, 2015]). Such a possibility could explain how in a parsimonious fashion 30 000 species of fish might have each come to have their own pheromones. How these sets of odors are discerned is not yet known; in particular, it is not known whether it might involve some type of highly prescribed learning that occurs as part of early development or even self-referencing such as that suggested for kin odor recognition (Ward, 2015). Whatever the specific circumstances (and there could be many given the diversity of fishes), there seems to be no doubt that shoaling odors are species-wide and adaptive and should be considered

    Enjoying the preview?
    Page 1 of 1