Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

Only $11.99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution
Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution
Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution
Ebook2,945 pages42 hours

Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars

()

Read preview

About this ebook

This comprehensive A to Z encyclopedia provides extensive coverage of important scientific terms related to improving our understanding of how we evolved. Specifically, the 5,000 entries in this two-volume set cover evidence and methods used to investigate the relationships among the living great apes, evidence about what makes the behavior of modern humans distinctive, and evidence about the evolutionary history of that distinctiveness, as well as information about modern methods used to trace the recent evolutionary history of modern human populations. This text provides a resource for everyone studying the emergence of Homo sapiens.

Visit the companion site www.woodhumanevolution.com to browse additional references and updates from this comprehensive encyclopedia.
LanguageEnglish
PublisherWiley
Release dateMar 31, 2011
ISBN9781444342475
Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution

Related to Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution

Related ebooks

Biology For You

View More

Related articles

Reviews for Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution

Rating: 0 out of 5 stars
0 ratings

0 ratings0 reviews

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

    Book preview

    Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution - Bernard Wood

    Contents

    Contributors

    Editor

    Executive Editor

    Assistant Executive Editor

    Senior Editorial Assistant

    Editorial Assistant

    Associate Editors

    Advisory Editors

    Section and Topic Editors

    Contributors

    Foreword

    Preface

    Antecedents

    Scope

    Organization

    Planning

    Delivery

    The future

    Errors

    Acknowledgments

    Topic Entry List

    Abbreviations

    Taxonomic conventions

    Informal taxonomic categories

    A

    B

    C

    D

    E

    F

    G

    H

    I

    J

    K

    L

    M

    N

    O

    P

    Q

    R

    S

    T

    U

    V

    W

    X

    Y

    Z

    References

    This encyclopedia is dedicated to the life and work of Clark Howell,

    Glynn Isaac, Charlie Lockwood, and Elizabeth Harmon.

    COMPANION WEBSITE

    This book has a website containing material to accompany the Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution:

    www.woodhumanevolution.com

    This paperback edition first published 2013 © 2011, 2013 by Blackwell Publishing Ltd

    Edition History: Blackwell Publishing Ltd (hardback, 2011)

    Registered Office

    John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex, PO19 8SQ, UK

    Editorial Offices

    9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK

    The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex, PO19 8SQ, UK

    111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030-5774, USA

    For details of our global editorial offices, for customer services and for information about how to apply for permission to reuse the copyright material in this book please see our website at www.wiley.com/wiley-blackwell.

    The right of the author to be identified as the author of this work has been asserted in accordance with the UK Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.

    All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by the UK Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, without the prior permission of the publisher.

    Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are often claimed as trademarks. All brand names and product names used in this book are trade names, service marks, trademarks or registered trademarks of their respective owners. The publisher is not associated with any product or vendor mentioned in this book.

    Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author(s) have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. It is sold on the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services and neither the publisher nor the author shall be liable for damages arising herefrom. If professional advice or other expert assistance is required, the services of a competent professional should be sought.

    Library of Congress Cataloguing-in-Publication data has been applied for

    ISBN: 978-1-1186-5099-8 (paperback)

    A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.

    Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in print may not be available in electronic books.

    Cover design by Design Deluxe

    Contributors

    Editor

    Wood, Bernard

    The George Washington University

    Executive Editor

    Henry, Amanda

    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

    Assistant Executive Editor

    Baker, Jennifer

    The George Washington University

    Senior Editorial Assistant

    Claxton, Alex

    Boston University

    Editorial Assistant

    Senjem, Jess

    University of Wisconsin

    Associate Editors

    Bishop, Laura

    Liverpool John Moores University

    Goodrum, Matthew

    Virginia Tech

    Tad, Schurr

    University of Pennsylvania

    Tryon, Christian

    New York University

    Advisory Editors

    Ackermann, Rebecca

    University of Cape Town

    Aiello, Leslie

    Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research, Inc.

    Antón, Susan

    New York University

    Barton, Nick

    University of Oxford

    Bilsborough, Alan

    Durham University

    Dean, Christopher

    University College London

    Dibble, Harold

    University of Pennsylvania

    Feibel, Craig

    Rutgers University

    Grine, Fred

    Stony Brook University

    Groves, Colin

    The Australian National University

    Hallgrimsson, Benedikt

    University of Calgary

    Harvati, Katerina

    Eberhard Karls Universitðt T bingen

    Hill, Andrew

    Yale University

    Hublin, Jean-Jacques

    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

    Kimbel, William

    Arizona State University

    Leakey, Meave

    Stony Brook University and Turkana Basin Institute

    Macchiarelli, Roberto

    Université de Poitiers

    Manzi, Giorgio

    University of Rome ‘La Sapienza’

    James O’Connell

    The University of Utah

    Pilbeam, David

    Harvard University

    Roche, Hélène

    CNRS, Université Paris Ouest Nanterre

    Rosas, Antonio

    Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales

    Smith, Fred

    Illinois State University

    Smith, Richard

    Washington University

    Stone, Anne

    Arizona State University

    Stringer, Chris

    Natural History Museum, London

    Thackeray, Francis

    University of the Witwatersrand

    Ward, Carol

    University of Missouri

    Zilhão, João

    ICREA/University of Barcelona

    Section and Topic Editors

    Bae, Christopher

    University of Hawaii

    Bailey, Shara

    New York University

    Collard, Mark

    Simon Fraser University

    Crowley, Brooke

    University of Cincinnati

    DeSilva, Jeremy

    Boston University

    Ditchfield, Peter

    University of Oxford

    Elton, Sarah

    Hull York Medical School

    Faith, Tyler

    The University of Queensland

    Feakins, Sarah

    University of Southern California

    Gordon, Adam

    University at Albany-SUNY

    Herries, Andy

    La Trobe University

    Kivell, Tracy

    University of Kent

    Konigsberg, Lyle

    University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

    Kramer, Andrew

    The University of Tennessee, Knoxville

    Moggi-Cecchi, Jacopo

    Università degli Studi di Firenze

    Pearson, Osbjorn

    University of New Mexico

    Petraglia, Michael

    University of Oxford

    Pickering, Robyn

    University of Melbourne

    Plummer, Tom

    The City University of New York

    Richmond, Brian

    The George Washington University

    Sherwood, Chet

    The George Washington University

    Smith, Tanya

    Harvard University

    Strait, David

    University at Albany-SUNY

    Subiaul, Francys

    The George Washington University

    Verna, Christine

    CNRS, Dynamique de l’Evolution Humaine, UPR2147, Paris

    Viola, Bence

    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

    Wang, Steve

    The City University of New York

    Contributors

    Adams, Justin

    Grand Valley State University

    Ashley, Gail

    Rutgers University

    Auerbach, Benjamin

    The University of Tennessee, Knoxville

    Bogart, Stephanie

    Agnes Scott College, Atlanta

    Brooks, Alison

    The George Washington University

    Claxton, Alex

    University of Indianapolis

    Constantino, Paul

    Marshall University

    Crevecoeur, Isabelle

    Université Bordeaux 1

    Crittenden, Alyssa

    University of California, San Diego

    Cunningham, Andrew

    Harvard University

    Dechow, Paul

    Texas A&M Health Science Center

    Despriée, Jackie

    National Museum of Natural History of Paris

    de Vos, John

    Netherlands Centre for Biodiversity Naturalis

    Dizon, Eusebio

    National Museum of the Philippines

    Domínguez-Rodrigo, Manuel

    Complutense University

    Drapeau, Michelle

    Université de Montréal

    Du, Andrew

    The George Washington University

    Dunsworth, Holly

    University of Rhode Island

    Farrell, Milly

    The Royal College of Surgeons of England

    Friedlaender, Jonathan

    Temple University

    Garcia Garriga, Joan

    Rovira i Virgili University-The Human Paleoecology and Social Evolution Institute

    Green, David

    The George Washington University

    Grosse, Ian

    University of Massachusetts, Amherst

    Gunz, Philipp

    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

    Higham, Tom

    Oxford Radiocarbon Accelerator Unit University of Oxford

    Johanson, Donald

    Arizona State University

    Joordens, Josephine

    Human Origins Group, Faculty of Archaeology, Leiden University

    Kelly, Robert

    University of Wyoming

    Kupczik, Kornelius

    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

    Lestrel, Pete

    7327 de Celis Place, Van Nuys, California 91406-2853

    Louchart, Antoine

    CNRS, ENS de Lyon

    McNulty, Kieran

    University of Minnesota

    McPherron, Shannon

    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

    MartÚnez Molina, Kenneth

    Rovira i Virgili University-The Human Paleoecology and Social Evolution Institute

    Martínez-Navarro, Bienvenido

    Institut Català de Paleoecología Humana i Evolució Social (IPHES) Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona

    Monge, Janet

    University of Pennsylvania

    Nelson, Emma

    University of Liverpool

    Patterson, David

    The George Washington University

    Quam, Rolf

    Binghamton University (SUNY)

    Pickering, Travis

    University of Wisconsin-Madison

    Potts, Richard

    National Museum of Natural History, The Smithsonian Institution

    Reynolds, Sally

    Liverpool John Moores University

    Ritzman, Terry

    Arizona State University

    Roebroeks, Wil

    Faculty of Archaeology Leiden University

    Ross, Callum

    The University of Chicago

    Schroer, Kes

    The George Washington University

    Sémah, Francois

    Muséum national d’histoire naturelle

    Sept, Jeanne

    Indiana University

    Setchell, Jo

    Durham University

    Skinner, Matt

    University College London

    Smith, Holly

    University of Michigan

    Speth, John

    University of Michigan

    Sponheimer, Matt

    University of Colorado at Boulder

    Spoor, Fred

    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

    Stanistreet, Ian

    University of Liverpool

    Steudel-Numbers, Karen

    University of Wisconsin-Madison

    Stewart, Kathlyn

    Canadian Museum of Nature

    Stout, Dietrich

    Emory University

    Thompson, Jennifer

    University of Nevada, Las Vegas

    Vogel, Erin

    The George Washington University

    Vonhof, Hubert

    VU University Amsterdam

    Willoughby, Pamela

    University of Edmonton

    Wynn, Jonathan

    University of South Florida

    Yellen, John

    National Science Foundation

    Zawidzki, Tadeusz

    The George Washington University

    Zipkin, Andrew

    The George Washington University

    Zonneveld, Frans

    Oranjestraat 35, Middelbeers, The Netherlands

    Foreword

    There is at present a consensus that the 21st century will be the century of biology, just as the 20th century was the century of physics. Biology now has larger budgets and a larger workforce than physics, and it faces problems of great significance and relevance to the understanding of human nature and the conduct of human life. The core of all biological research and understanding is the theory of evolution; evolution by natural selection is the central unifying concept of biology. As the great 20th century evolutionist Theodosius Dobzhansky asserted in 1973, Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution.

    The theory of evolution has transformed our understanding of life on planet Earth. Evolution provides a scientific explanation for why there are so many different kinds of organisms and why they all share the same chemical components in similar proportions, and why all organisms share DNA as their hereditary material, and why enzymes and other proteins, which are the fundamental constituents and engines of cell processes, are all made up of the same 20 amino acids, despite hundreds of amino acids existing in organisms. Evolution demonstrates why some organisms that look quite different are in fact related, while other organisms that may look similar are only distantly related. It accounts for the origins of humans on Earth and reveals our species’ biological connections with other living things in varying degrees. Evolution explains the similarities and differences among modern human groups and modern human individuals. It enables the development of effective new ways to protect ourselves against constantly evolving bacteria and viruses, and to improve the quality of our agricultural products and domestic animals.

    We owe the concept of evolution by natural selection to Charles Darwin. Natural selection was proposed by Darwin primarily to account for the adaptive organization, or design, of living beings; it is a process that preserves and promotes adaptation. Evolutionary change through time and evolutionary diversification (multiplication of species) often ensue as byproducts of natural selection fostering the adaptation of organisms to their milieu. Evolutionary change is not directly promoted by natural selection and, therefore, it is not its necessary consequence. Indeed, some species remain unchanged for long periods of time, as Darwin noted. Nautilus, Lingula, and other so-called living fossils were used by Darwin as examples of organisms that have remained unchanged in their appearance for millions of years.

    Evolution affects all aspects of an organism’s life: morphology (form and structure), physiology (function), behavior, and ecology (interaction with the environment). Underlying these changes are changes in the hereditary materials. Hence, in genetic terms, evolution consists of changes in an organism’s hereditary makeup and can be seen as a two-step process. First, hereditary variation arises by mutation; second, selection occurs by which useful variations increase in frequency and those that are less useful or injurious are eliminated over the generations. As Darwin (The Origin of Species, 1859) saw it, individuals having useful variations would have the best chance of surviving and procreating their kind (p. 81). As a consequence, useful variations increase in frequency over the generations, at the expense of those that are less useful or are injurious.

    Natural selection is much more than a purifying process, for it is able to generate novelty by increasing the probability of otherwise extremely improbable genetic combinations. Natural selection in combination with mutation becomes, in this respect, a creative process. Moreover, it is a process that has been occurring for many millions of years, in many different evolutionary lineages, and in a multitude of species, each consisting of a large number of individuals. Evolution by mutation and natural selection has produced the enormous diversity of the living world with its wondrous adaptations and it accounts for our presence, Homo sapiens, on planet Earth.

    There is a version of the history of ideas that sees a parallel between two scientific revolutions, the Copernican and the Darwinian. In this view, the Copernican Revolution consisted of displacing the Earth from its previously accepted locus as the center of the universe, moving it to a subordinate place as just one more planet revolving around the sun. Similarly, in a congruous manner, the Darwinian Revolution is viewed as consisting of the displacement of modern humans from their exalted position as the center of life on Earth, with all other species created for the service of humankind. According to this version of intellectual history, Copernicus had accomplished his revolution with the heliocentric theory of the solar system. Darwin’s achievement emerged from his theory of organic evolution. (Sigmund Freud refers to these two revolutions as outrages inflicted upon humankind’s self-image and adds a third one, his own. He sees psychoanalysis as the third and most bitter blow upon man’s craving for grandiosity, revealing that man’s ego is not even master in his own house.)

    What the standard versions of the Copernican and Darwinian revolutions say is correct but inadequate. It misses what is most important about these two intellectual revolutions, namely that they ushered in the beginning of science in the modern sense. These two revolutions may jointly be seen as the one Scientific Revolution, with two stages, the Copernican and the Darwinian.

    The Copernican Revolution was launched with the publication in 1543, the year of Nicolaus Copernicus’ death, of his De revolutionibus orbium celestium (On the Revolutions of the Celestial Spheres), and it bloomed in 1687 with the publication of Isaac Newton’s Philosophiae naturalis principia mathematica (The Mathematical Principles of Natural Philosophy). The discoveries by Copernicus, Kepler, Galileo, Newton, and others, in the 16th and 17th centuries, had shown that Earth is not the center of the universe, but is a small planet rotating around an average star; that the universe is immense in space and in time; and that the motions of the planets around the sun can be explained by the same simple laws that account for the motion of physical objects on our planet. These include laws such as f = m × a (force = mass×acceleration) or the inverse-square law of attraction, f = g(m1m2)/r² (the force of attraction between two bodies is directly proportional to their masses, but inversely related to the square of the distance between them).

    These and other discoveries greatly expanded modern human knowledge. The conceptual revolution they brought about was more fundamental yet: a commitment to the postulate that the universe obeys immanent laws that account for natural phenomena. The workings of the universe were brought into the realm of science: explanation through natural laws. Potentially, all physical phenomena could be accounted for, as long as the causes were adequately known.

    The advances of physical science brought about by the Copernican Revolution had driven humankind’s conception of the universe to a split-personality state of affairs, a condition that persisted well into the mid-19th century. Scientific explanations, derived from natural laws, dominated the world of nonliving matter, on the Earth as well as in the heavens. Supernatural explanations, which depended on the unfathomable deeds of the creator, were accepted as explanations of the origin and configuration of living creatures. Authors, such as William Paley in his Natural Theology (1802), had developed the argument from design, the notion that the complex design of organisms could not have come about by chance, or by the mechanical laws of physics, chemistry, and astronomy, but was rather accomplished by an omnipotent deity, just as the complexity of a watch, designed to tell time, was accomplished by an intelligent watchmaker.

    Darwin completed the Copernican Revolution by drawing out for biology the notion of nature as a lawful system of matter in motion that modern human reason can explain without recourse to supernatural agencies. Darwin’s greatest accomplishment was to show that the complex organization and functionality of living beings can be explained as the result of a natural process: natural selection. The origin and adaptations of organisms in their profusion and wondrous variations were thus brought into the realm of science.

    Darwin’s theory of evolution by natural selection disposed of Paley’s arguments: the adaptations of organisms are not outcomes of chance, but of a process that, over time, causes the gradual accumulation of features beneficial to organisms. There is design in the living world: eyes are designed for seeing, wings for flying, and kidneys for regulating the composition of the blood. But the design of organisms is not intelligent, as would be expected from an engineer, but imperfect and worse: defects, dysfunctions, oddities, waste, and cruelty pervade the living world. Darwin’s focus in The Origin of Species (1859) was the explanation of design, with evolution playing the subsidiary role of supporting evidence.

    It follows from Darwin’s explanation of adaptation that evolution must necessarily occur as a consequence of organisms becoming adapted to different environments in different localities, and to the ever-changing conditions of the environment over time, and as hereditary variations become available at a particular time that improve, in that place and at that time, the organisms’ chances of survival and reproduction. Origin’s evidence for biological evolution is central to Darwin’s explanation of design, because this explanation implies that biological evolution occurs, which Darwin therefore seeks to demonstrate in the second half of the book.

    Darwin and other 19th century biologists found compelling evidence for biological evolution in the comparative study of living organisms, in their geographic distribution, and in the fossil remains of extinct organisms. Since Darwin’s time, the evidence from these sources has become stronger and more comprehensive, while biological disciplines that have emerged recently – genetics, biochemistry, ecology, animal behavior (ethology), neurobiology, and especially molecular biology – have supplied powerful additional evidence and detailed confirmation. Accordingly, evolutionists are no longer concerned with obtaining evidence to support the fact of evolution, but rather are concerned with finding out additional information of the historical process in cases of particular interest. Moreover and most importantly, evolutionists nowadays are interested in understanding further and further how the process of evolution occurs.

    Nevertheless, important discoveries continue, even in traditional disciplines, such as paleontology. Skeptical contemporaries of Darwin asked about the missing links, particularly between the extant apes and modern humans, but also between major groups of organisms, such as between fish and terrestrial tetrapods or between reptiles and birds. Evolutionists can now affirm that these missing links are no longer missing. The known fossil record has made great strides over the last century and a half. Many fossils intermediate between diverse organisms have been discovered over the years. Two examples are Archaeopteryx, an animal intermediate between reptiles and birds, and Tiktaalik, intermediate between fishes and tetrapods.

    The missing link between apes and humans is not, either, missing any longer. Not one, but hundreds of fossil remains from hundreds of individual hominins have been discovered since Darwin’s time and continue to be discovered at an accelerated rate. The history of hominin discoveries is narrated in this encyclopedia, as well as the anatomical and other changes that occur through time.

    Darwin wrote two books dedicated to human evolution: The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (2 vols, 1871) and The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (1872). What we now know about human evolution is immensely more than what Darwin knew. But, even concerning hominin fossil history, much remains to be discovered. Indeed, the sequence that goes from the most primitive hominins to Homo sapiens, our species, is not resolved. That is, in many cases we do not know whether a particular hominin fossil belongs to the line of descent that goes to our species, or whether it belongs to a lateral branch.

    There are many other important issues concerning the evolutionary origin of modern human traits – anatomical, physiological, behavioral, and cultural – that remain largely unknown. I will briefly point out three great research frontiers that seem to me particularly significant: ontogenic decoding, the brain/mind puzzle, and the ape-to-human transformation. By ontogenetic decoding I refer to the problem of how the unidimensional genetic information encoded in the DNA of a single cell becomes transformed into a four-dimensional being, the individual that develops, grows, matures, and dies. Cancer, disease, and aging are epiphenomena of ontogenetic decoding. By the brain/mind puzzle I refer to the interdependent questions of (a) how the physicochemical signals that reach our sense organs become transformed into perceptions, feelings, ideas, critical arguments, aesthetic emotions, and ethical values; and (b) how, out of this diversity of experiences, there emerges a unitary reality, the mind or self. Free will and language, social and political institutions, technology, and art are all epiphenomena of the modern human mind. By the ape-to-human transformation I refer to the mystery of how a particular ape lineage became a hominin lineage, from which emerged, after only a few million years, modern humans able to think and love, who have developed complex societies and uphold ethical, aesthetic, and religious values. But the modern human genome differs little from the chimp genome.

    I will refer to these three issues as the egg-to-adult transformation, the brain-to-mind transformation, and the ape-to-human transformation. The egg-to-adult transformation is essentially similar, and similarly mysterious, in modern humans and other mammals, but it has distinctive human features. The brain-to-mind transformation and the ape-to-human transformation are distinctively human. These three transformations define the humanum, that which makes us specifically modern human. Few other issues in human evolution are of greater consequence for understanding ourselves and our place in nature.

    The instructions that guide the ontogenetic process, or the egg-to-adult transformation, are carried in the hereditary material. The theory of biological heredity was formulated by the Augustinian monk Gregor Mendel in 1866, but it became generally known by biologists only in 1900: genetic information is contained in discrete factors, or genes, which exist in pairs, one received from each parent. The next step toward understanding the nature of genes was completed during the first quarter of the twentieth century. It was established that genes are parts of the chromosomes, filamentous bodies present in the nucleus of the cell, and that they are linearly arranged along the chromosomes. It took another quarter century to determine the chemical composition of genes: deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA). DNA consists of four kinds of nucleotides organized in long, double-helical structures. The genetic information is contained in the linear sequence of the nucleotides, very much in the same way as the semantic information of an English sentence is conveyed by the particular sequence of the 26 letters of the alphabet.

    The first important step toward understanding how the genetic information is decoded came in 1941 when George W. Beadle and Edward L. Tatum demonstrated that genes determine the synthesis of enzymes; enzymes are the catalysts that control all chemical reactions in living beings. Later it became known that amino acids (the components that make up enzymes and other proteins) are encoded, each by a set of three consecutive nucleotides. This relationship accounts for the linear correspondence between a particular sequence of coding nucleotides and the sequence of the amino acids that make up the encoded enzyme.

    Chemical reactions in organisms must occur in an orderly manner; organisms must have ways of switching genes on and off since different sets of genes are active in different cells. The first control system was discovered in 1961 by François Jacob and Jacques Monod for a gene that encodes an enzyme that digests sugar in the bacterium Escherichia coli. The gene is turned on and off by a system of several switches consisting of short DNA sequences adjacent to the coding part of the gene. (The coding sequence of a gene is the part that determines the sequence of amino acids in the encoded enzyme or protein.) The switches acting on a given gene are activated or deactivated by feedback loops that involve molecules synthesized by other genes. A variety of gene control mechanisms were soon discovered, in bacteria and other micro-organisms. Two elements are typically present: feedback loops and short DNA sequences acting as switches. The feedback loops ensure that the presence of a substance in the cell induces the synthesis of the enzyme required to digest it, and that an excess of the enzyme in the cell represses its own synthesis. (For example, the gene encoding a sugar-digesting enzyme in E. coli is turned on or off by the presence or absence of the sugar to be digested.)

    The investigation of gene-control mechanisms in mammals (and other complex organisms) became possible in the mid-1970s with the development of recombinant DNA techniques. This technology made it feasible to isolate single genes (and other DNA sequences) and to multiply them, or clone them, to obtain the quantities necessary for ascertaining their nucleotide sequence. One unanticipated discovery was that most genes come in pieces: the coding sequence of a gene is divided into several fragments separated one from the next by noncoding DNA segments. In addition to the alternating succession of coding and noncoding segments, mammalian genes contain short control sequences, like those in bacteria but typically more numerous and complex, that act as control switches and signal where the coding sequence begins.

    Much remains to be discovered about the control mechanisms of mammalian genes. The daunting speed at which molecular biology is advancing has led to the discovery of some prototypes of mammalian gene control systems, but much remains to be unraveled. Moreover, understanding the control mechanisms of individual genes is but the first major step toward solving the mystery of ontogenetic decoding. The second major step will is the puzzle of differentiation.

    A modern human being consists of one trillion cells of some 300 different kinds, all derived by sequential division from the fertilized egg, a single cell 0.1 mm in diameter. The first few cell divisions yield a spherical mass of amorphous cells. Successive divisions are accompanied by the appearance of folds and ridges in the mass of cells and, later on, of the variety of tissues, organs, and limbs characteristic of a human individual. The full complement of genes duplicates with each cell division, so that two complete genomes are present in every cell. Yet different sets of genes are active in different cells. This must be so for cells to differentiate: a nerve cell, a muscle cell, and a skin cell are vastly different in size, configuration, and function. The differential activity of genes must continue after differentiation, because different cells fulfill different functions, which are controlled by different genes. Nevertheless, experiments with other animals (and some with humans) indicate that all the genes in any cell have the potential of becoming activated. (The sheep Dolly was conceived using the genes extracted from a cell in an adult sheep.)

    The information that controls cell and organ differentiation is ultimately contained in the DNA sequence, but mostly in very short segments of it. In mammals, insects, and other complex organisms there are control circuits that operate at higher levels than the control mechanisms that activate and deactivate individual genes. These higher-level circuits (such as the so-called homeobox genes) act on sets of genes rather than individual genes. The details of how these sets are controlled, how many control systems there are, and how they interact, as well as many other related questions, are what needs to be resolved to elucidate the egg-to-adult transformation. The DNA sequence of some controlling elements has been ascertained, but this is a minor effort that is only helped a little by plowing the way through the entire 3000 million nucleotide pairs that constitute the modern human genome. Experiments with stem cells are likely to provide important knowledge as scientists ascertain how stem cells become brain cells in one case, muscle cells in another, and so on.

    The benefits that the elucidation of the egg-to-adult transformation will bring to humankind are enormous. This knowledge will make possible understanding the modes of action of complex genetic diseases, including cancer, and therefore their cure. It will also bring an understanding of the process of aging, which kills all those who have won the battle against other infirmities.

    Cancer is an anomaly of ontogenetic decoding: cells proliferate despite the welfare of the organism demanding otherwise. Individual genes (oncogenes) have been identified that are involved in the causation of particular forms of cancer. But whether or not a cell will turn out cancerous depends on the interaction of the oncogenes with other genes and with the internal and external environment of the cell. Aging is also a failure of the process of ontogenetic decoding: cells fail to carry out the functions imprinted in their genetic code script or are no longer able to proliferate and replace dead cells.

    The brain is the most complex and most distinctive modern human organ. It consists of 30 billion nerve cells, or neurons, each connected to many others through two kinds of cell extension, known as the axon and the dendrites. From the evolutionary point of view, the animal brain is a powerful biological adaptation; it allows the organism to obtain and process information about environmental conditions and then to adapt to them. This ability has been carried to the limit in modern humans, in which the extravagant hypertrophy of the brain makes possible abstract thinking, language, and technology. By these means, humankind has ushered in a new mode of adaptation far more powerful than the biological mode: adaptation by culture.

    The most rudimentary ability to gather and process information about the environment is found in certain single-celled micro-organisms. The protozoan Paramecium swims apparently at random, ingesting the bacteria it encounters, but when it meets unsuitable acidity or salinity its advance is checked and it starts off in a new direction. The single-celled alga Euglena not only avoids unsuitable environments but seeks suitable ones by orienting itself according to the direction of light, which it perceives through a light-sensitive spot in the cell. Plants have not progressed much further. Except for those with tendrils that twist around any solid object and the few carnivorous plants that react to touch, they mostly react only to gradients of light, gravity, and moisture.

    In animals the ability to secure and process environmental information is mediated by the nervous system. The simplest nervous systems are found in corals and jellyfishes; they lack coordination between different parts of their bodies, so any one part is able to react only when it is directly stimulated. Sea urchins and starfish possess a nerve ring and radial nerve cords that coordinate stimuli coming from different parts; hence, they respond with direct and unified actions of the whole body. They have no brain, however, and seem unable to learn from experience. Planarian flatworms have the most rudimentary brain known; their central nervous system and brain process and coordinate information gathered by sensory cells. These animals are capable of simple learning and hence of variable responses to repeatedly encountered stimuli. Insects and their relatives have much more advanced brains; they obtain precise chemical, acoustic, visual, and tactile signals from the environment and process them, making possible complex behaviors, particularly in search for food, selection of mates, and social organization.

    Vertebrates are able to obtain and process much more complicated signals and to respond to the environment more variably than do insects or any other type of invertebrate. The vertebrate brain contains an enormous number of associative neurons arranged in complex patterns. In vertebrates the ability to react to environmental information is correlated with an increase in the relative size of the cerebral hemispheres and of the neopallium, an organ involved in associating and coordinating signals from all receptors and brain centers. In mammals, the neopallium has expanded and become the cerebral cortex. Modern humans have a very large brain relative to their body size, and a cerebral cortex that is disproportionately large and complex even for their brain size. Abstract thinking, symbolic language, complex social organization, values, and ethics are manifestations of the wondrous capacity of the modern human brain to gather information about the external world and to integrate that information and react flexibly to what is perceived.

    With the advanced development of the modern human brain, biological evolution has transcended itself, opening up a new mode of evolution: adaptation by technological manipulation of the environment. Organisms adapt to the environment by means of natural selection, by changing their genetic constitution over the generations to suit the demands of the environment. Modern humans, and modern humans alone to any substantial degree, have developed the capacity to adapt to hostile environments by modifying the environments according to the needs of their genes. The discovery of fire and the fabrication of clothing and shelter have allowed the ancestors of modern humans to spread from the warm tropical and subtropical regions of the Old World, to which we are biologically adapted, to almost the whole Earth; it was not necessary for wandering hominins that they wait until genes would evolve providing anatomical protection against cold temperatures by changing their physiology or by means of fur or hair. Nor are modern humans biding their time in expectation of wings or gills; we have conquered the air and seas with artfully designed contrivances: airplanes and ships. It is the modern human brain (the human mind) that has made humankind the most successful, by most meaningful standards, living species.

    There are not enough bits of information in the complete DNA sequence of a modern human genome to specify the trillions of connections among the 30 billion neurons of the modern human brain. Accordingly, the genetic instructions must be organized in control circuits operating at different hierarchical levels so that an instruction at one level is carried through many channels at a lower level in the hierarchy of control circuits. The development of the modern human brain is indeed one particularly intriguing component of the egg-to-adult transformation.

    Within the last two decades, neurobiology has developed into one of the most exciting biological disciplines. An increased commitment of financial and human resources has brought an unprecedented rate of discovery. Much has been learned about how light, sound, temperature, resistance, and chemical impressions received in our sense organs trigger the release of chemical transmitters and electric potential differences that carry the signals through the nerves to the brain and elsewhere in the body. Much has also been learned about how neural channels for information transmission become reinforced by use or may be replaced after damage; about which neurons or groups of neurons are committed to processing information derived from a particular organ or environmental location; and about many other matters. But, for all this progress, neurobiology remains an infant discipline, at a stage of theoretical development comparable perhaps to that of genetics at the beginning of the 20th century. Those things that count most remain shrouded in mystery: how physical phenomena become mental experiences (the feelings and sensations, called qualia by philosophers, that contribute the elements of consciousness), and how out of the diversity of these experiences emerges the mind, a reality with unitary properties, such as free will and the awareness of self, that persist through an individual’s life.

    I do not believe that the mysteries of the mind are unfathomable; rather, they are puzzles that modern humans can solve with the methods of science and illuminate with philosophical analysis and reflection. And I will place my bets that, over the next half century or so, many of these puzzles will be solved. We shall then be well on our way toward answering the biblical injunction: Know thyself.

    A contemporary development that would have greatly delighted Darwin is the determination of the DNA sequence of the modern human genome, an investigation that was started under the label, the Human Genome Project, which opens up the possibility of comparing the modern human DNA sequence with that of other organisms, observing their similarities and differences, seeking to ascertain the changes in the DNA that account for distinctively modern human features. The Human Genome Project was initiated in 1989, funded through two US agencies, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and the Department of Energy (DOE), with eventual participation of scientists outside the USA. The goal set was to obtain the complete sequence of one human genome in 15 years at an approximate cost of $3000 million, coincidentally about $1 per DNA letter. A private enterprise, Celera Genomics, started in the USA somewhat later, but joined the government-sponsored project in achieving, largely independently, similar results at about the same time. A draft of the genome sequence was completed ahead of schedule in 2001. In 2003 the Human Genome Project was finished, but the analysis of the DNA sequences chromosome by chromosome continued over the following years. Results of these detailed analyses were published on June 1, 2006. The draft DNA sequence of the chimpanzee genome was published on September 1, 2005 (an entry in the encyclopedia presents the first fossil evidence of panins ever to be discovered). In the regions of the genome that are shared by modern humans and chimpanzees the two species are about 99% identical. These differences may seem very small or quite large, depending on how one chooses to look at them: 1% is only a small fraction of the total, but it still amounts to a difference of 30 million DNA nucleotides out of the 3 billion in each genome.

    Twenty-nine percent of the enzymes and other proteins encoded by the genes are identical in both species. Out of the one hundred to several hundred amino acids that make up each protein, the 71% of nonidentical proteins that differ between modern humans and chimps do so by only two amino acids, on average. If one takes into account DNA stretches found in one species but not the other, the two genomes are about 96% identical, rather than nearly 99% identical as in the case of DNA sequences shared by both species. That is, a large amount of genetic material, about 3% or some 90 million DNA nucleotides, have been inserted or deleted since ancestors went their separate evolutionary ways, about 8–6 million years ago. Most of this DNA does not contain genes coding for proteins, although it may include toolkit genes and switch genes that impact developmental processes, as the rest of the noncoding DNA surely does.

    Comparison of the modern human and chimpanzee genomes provides insights into the rate of evolution of particular genes in the two species. One significant finding is that genes active in the brain have changed more in the human lineage than in the chimp lineage (Khaitovich et al. 2005). Also significant is that the fastest-evolving modern human genes are those coding for transcription factors. These are switch proteins which control the expression of other genes; that is, they determine when other genes are turned on and off. On the whole, 585 genes have been identified as evolving faster in humans than in chimps, including genes involved in resistance to malaria and tuberculosis. (It might be mentioned that malaria is a severe disease for humans but much less so for chimps.)

    Genes located on the Y chromosome, found only in the male, have been much better protected by natural selection in the human than in the chimpanzee lineage, in which several genes have incorporated disabling mutations that make the genes nonfunctional. Also, there are several regions of the human genome that contain beneficial genes that have rapidly evolved within the past 250,000 years. One region contains the FOXP2 gene, involved in the evolution of speech.

    Other regions that show higher rates of evolution in humans than in chimpanzees and other animals include 49 segments, dubbed human accelerated regions or HARs. The greatest observed difference occurs in HAR1F, an RNA gene that is expressed specifically in Cajal-Retzius neurons in the developing human neocortex from 7 to 19 gestational weeks, a crucial period for cortical neuron specification and migration.

    Extended comparisons of the human and chimpanzee genomes and experimental exploration of the functions associated with significant genes will surely advance further our understanding, over the next decade or two, of what it is that makes us distinctively human, what is it that differentiates H. sapiens from our closest living species, chimpanzees and bonobos, and will surely shine some light on how and when these differences may have come about during hominin evolution of the human species. Surely also, full biological understanding of the ape-to-human transformation will only come if we solve the other two conundrums: the egg-to-adult transformation and the brain-to-mind transformation. The distinctive features that make us modern human appear early in development, well before birth, as the linear information encoded in the genome gradually becomes expressed into a four-dimensional individual, an individual who changes in configuration as time goes by. In an important sense the most distinctive human features are those expressed in the brain, those that account for the human mind or for human identity.

    Francisco J. Ayala

    University of California, Irvine

    Preface

    Once upon a time, say 60 years ago, the only background required to appreciate what was known about human evolution was a familiarity with a relatively sparse fossil record, an understanding of the limited information there was about the context of the sites, some knowledge of gross anatomy, a familiarity with a few simple analytical methods, and an appreciation of general evolutionary principles.

    Times have changed. The fossil record has grown exponentially, imaging techniques allow researchers to capture previously unavailable gross morphological and microstructural evidence in unimaginable quantities, analytical methods have burgeoned in scope and complexity, phylogeny reconstruction is more sophisticated, molecular biology has revolutionized our understanding of genetics, evolutionary history, modern human variation, and development, and a host of different developments in biology, chemistry, earth sciences, and physics the have enriched evidence about the biotic, climatic, and temporal context of the hominin fossil record. In short, the fossil evidence and the range of methods used to study human evolution have grown by at least one order of magnitude in the past six decades. Yet there is no single reference source where students and researchers involved in human evolution research, be they archeologists, earth scientists, molecular biologists, morphologists, paleontologists, or paleoecologists, can go to find out about topics as diverse as C4 plants canalization, the candelabra model, canonical variates analysis, Carabelli(‘s) trait, carcass transport strategy, cardioid foramen magnum, carrying capacity, and Caune de l’Arago.

    Antecedents

    Quenstedt and Quenstedt (1936) were probably the first to compile a comprehensive list and associated bibliography of the hominin fossil record. Hue (1937) illustrated some of the hominin fossil evidence, but his bibliographic coverage was less comprehensive than Quenstedt and Quenstedt’s. Vallois and Movius’ Catalogue des Hommes Fossiles (1953) made a commendable attempt to survey the human fossil record, as did Day’s Guide to Fossil Man (1965). The first comprehensive attempt to collate information about both the hominin fossil record and its context came in the late 1960s and thereafter with the publication of the British Museum (Natural History)’s Catalogue of Fossil Hominids. The first volume, Part I: Africa, edited by Oakley and Campbell, was published in 1967. Subsequent volumes covered fossil hominin discoveries from Europe (Oakley, Campbell, and Molleson 1971) and Asia (Oakley, Campbell, and Molleson 1975). Unfortunately (but understandably given the work involved in assembling a resource such as this in a non-electronic format and on a rapidly enlarging hominin fossil record) only Part I ran to a second updated edition (Oakley, Campbell, and Molleson 1977) and the series has been discontinued. The most recent attempt to take up the challenge of providing a comprehensive catalogue of the fossil evidence for hominin evolution has been the initiative by Orban and her colleagues, entitled Hominid Remains – an up-date. This is distributed as part of a general series of publications called the Supplements au Bulletin de la Societe Royale Belge d’Anthropologie et de Prehistoire. The first in the series of separate publications was published in the beginning of 1988 and publication has continued thereafter. Each slim volume covers one or more countries and retains the format of the Catalogue of Fossil Hominids. The early issues were organized by an Editor (Orban) and Associate Editors (Slachmuylder, Semal, and Alewaeters). The individual entries are put together by experts who are familiar with one or more temporal phases of the hominin fossil record from that country or countries. Spencer’s excellent History of Physical Anthropology, published in 1997, is an important and authoritative source of information about the history of paleoanthropology, but although it has entries for the major fossil sites there are few entries about the fossil and archeological evidence. The four-volume series The Human Fossil Record edited by Jeffrey Schwartz and Ian Tatterall and colleagues, focuses on the better-preserved hominin fossils, but there is little about their context and its taxonomic interpretations are idiosyncratic. There is also a Catalogue of Fossil Hominids available on the internet at http://gbs.ur-plaza.osaka-cu.ac.jp/kaseki.

    Scope

    The long-term plan for the Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution (W-BEHE) is that it will be an authoritative and accessible source of information about the hominin clade of the tree of life. Entries cover:

    general evolutionary principles,

    information about the molecular and developmental biological approaches used to help understand the pattern and process of evolution,

    methods used to investigate relationships among the living great apes and modern humans,

    methods germane to understanding the origins and evolutionary history of the hominin clade and its climatic and ecological context,

    what makes the behavior of modern humans distinctive and the evolutionary history of that distinctiveness,

    information about the modern methods used to capture and interpret data from the hominin fossil and archeological records, as well as comparative biology,

    nonhominin fossil evidence germane to the evolution of the hominin clade,

    specialist terms used to describe the hominin fossil and archeological records,

    hypotheses germane to interpreting human evolutionary history,

    biographies of individuals who have made significant contributions to the accumulation of the fossil and archeological, and other, evidence and to its interpretation,

    institutions and organizations that have contributed to our understanding of human evolutionary history,

    information about hominin fossil and comparative great ape collections and about some of the repositories that hold hominin fossil collections.

    Organization

    The entries for the important fossil and/or archeological sites (later editions will aim to cover all the relevant sites) relevant to hominin evolution are structured to make it easier for the reader to find out what they want to know. This is also the case for the entries for the more complete hominin fossils, and for some less complete fossils that have a particular significance. There are also formally structured entries for taxa that have been used to accommodate the hominin fossil record. The depth of the entries varies; the entries for fossils and sites do not aim to be exhaustive, but they are comprehensive. The entries for methods and important biological principles try to explain complex concepts in plain and simple language and where possible they include examples. As Editor I constantly needed to remind myself, the editors, and the contributors that the W-BEHE is not an encyclopedia of archeology, earth science, genetics, etc.; readers should go to other sources for technical information about the methods.

    The entries have been prepared as a collective exercise. The drafts prepared by an editor or contributor were all screened by me as Editor for depth, style, and intelligibility and then sent to other editors, or to outside experts, for their comments. Human evolution is a controversial field, so where appropriate entries describe competing hypotheses, and if the entry comes down in favor of one of them, the entry explains why. The aim, doubtless not always met, was that the entries are self-contained and intelligible; we did not want users of the W-BEHE to look up one term they did not understand only to be faced with an entry that uses five words they are unfamiliar with. Within the text of entries, where appropriate, any term used that has its own entry are shown in bold.

    Planning

    The draft list of entries was drawn up by the Editor, four Section Editors (Laura Bishop, paleontology; Craig Feibel, earth sciences; Tom Plummer, archeology; and Anne Stone, molecular biology and genetics) and 17 Topic Editors (Shara Bailey, Robin Bernstein, Mark Collard, Sarah Elton, Matthew Goodrum, Adam Gordon, Katerina Harvati, Lyle Konigsberg, Andrew Kramer, Jacopo Moggi-Cecchi, Osbjorn Pearson, Brian Richmond, Chet Sherwood, Tanya Smith, David Strait, Francys Subiaul, and Christian Tryon). Important advice at this stage also came from several of the Advisory Editors.

    The original plan was that these individuals would prepare the entries, but like all strategic plans this one had to be modified. It was clear that the sheer volume of the work involved could not be tackled by these editors, and as polymathic as many of them are, there were still regions of the world (e.g., China) and topics (e.g., paleoclimate, stable isotopes, zooarcheology, etc.) for which we lacked coverage. Also, because this project has been 6 years in the making, circumstances change (children are born, grants are written and awarded, relatives get sick, individuals take up additional responsibilities) so I soon learned that it was adapt or die! Thus, the original editor classification has been abandoned and in its place there are now Associate Editors, Section and Topic Editors, and Contributors. The criteria for allocating individuals to categories are inevitably subjective and I am also conscious that my memory is fallible, so I offer my abject apologies to those who feel their contributions have not been recognized at the appropriate level. The Associate Editors did all, or some, of the following. They were the lead author on a substantial number of entries, they contributed generously to the editing well beyond their immediate area of expertise, and they responded promptly and generously to my many unreasonable requests. Only I (and they) know just how hard they worked and how important their contribution was. Section Editors did all, or some, of the above, but unlike the Associate Editors their contributions were focused on their immediate area of expertise. Topic Editors were much like Section Editors, but they contributed fewer entries and their contributions were confined to their immediate area of expertise. Contributors contributed at least one entry, but were not involved in the editing. Several Advisory Editors (e.g., Rebecca Ackermann, Leslie Aiello, Chris Dean, Colin Groves, Benedikt Hallgrimsson, and Bill Kimbel) contributed entries and some of the above played a major editorial role. Many other people offered advice about entries and they are listed in the Acknowledgments; once again my apologies if my system for recording such help has failed. Please, if you do not see your name on that list and you think you should be on it, please do not be bashful about letting me know and I can rectify any omissions in the next edition.

    Delivery

    I urge those of you who have not read Simon Winchester’s The Meaning of Everything to do so; those of you who have read it will know the Oxford English Dictionary took 70 years to complete. Much of the information in the W-BEHE is time-sensitive and the problem is that if you wait too long for that sincerely promised but much delayed entry you run the risk that many other entries will become outdated in the meantime. Yet you cannot go the press with an encyclopedia that is lacking essential entries. So we had to make a fine judgment about when to stop, and we had to close the headword list knowing we would be leaving some fossil, site, museum, and biographical entries for a later edition. You cannot have an encyclopedia without entries, but you can have entries without an encyclopedia. My efforts as Editor, and those of the Associate, Section, and Topic Editors and the Contributors, would have all come to naught without the hard work of the Executive Editors (Amanda Henry and Jennifer Baker) and the Editorial Assistants (Alex Claxton and Erin Mikels), who helped prepare the material for copy-editing and then checked it at various stages in the process my sincere thanks to all of them. Of course the buck stops with the Editor, so any mistakes are my responsibility alone.

    The future

    The plan is to follow up the hardback library edition with a more affordable edition intended for individuals, an abridged version for students, and an electronic edition that can be updated approximately every 6 months.

    Errors

    In a project of this scale errors will have been made. The only way to eliminate as many mistakes as humanly possible is to contact me (bernardawood@gmail.com) and I will see to it they are rectified in later editions.

    Bernard Wood

    The George Washington University, Washington, DC

    December 2010

    Acknowledgments

    It goes without saying that none of this would have been possible without the editors and contributors. At many times in the process it must have seemed as if it was all grit and no gravy and I am conscious that contributing in various ways to the Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution (W-BEHE) has deflected individuals from their core activities of teaching, grant writing, research, and writing up their research. I am more grateful to them than I can express in words. Some contributions deserve special mention. A few people really did read and comment on the entries I sent out for general review, and I am especially grateful to Rebecca Ackermann, Chris Dean, and Alan Bilsborough for consistently doing so. I am also grateful to Katerina Harvati for her support despite having other much more important claims on her time. Colin Groves (in Australia) would be getting up as I was going to sleep, but I cannot recall how many times I would send him complex requests and enquiries at the end of my day and when I woke up they would invariably be answered thoroughly and cheerfully. I am also immensely impressed by the graduate students who gave of their time and expertise; if Jennifer Baker, Amy Bauernfeind, Serena Bianchi, Habiba Chirchir, Andrew Du, Tyler Faith, Liz Renner, Kes Schroer, Steve Wang, and Andrew Zipkin are representative of young researchers then our discipline is in good hands. Lastly, for those who cherish such achievements, Tyler Faith and Christian Tryon tied for the shortest interval (c.18 minutes) between a request and a received entry.

    This project could not have been completed without the generosity of the community of scholars who work in paleoanthropology and in cognate areas. People who helped prepare and edit entries include undergraduates, junior and senior graduate students, postdoctoral fellows, and junior and senior professors; some probably felt they had no choice, others I cold-called and some I have known for 40 years. The willingness of people, young and old, to work for the common good has been humbling. It is inadequate thanks to list them by name, but that is the currency I must, perforce, use. Some of them devoted substantial time and effort to make sure that entries, especially site entries, were correct; I hope they conclude that the product of their collective labors justifies their efforts: Zeray Alemseged, Juan Luis Arsuaga, Lucinda Backwell, Amy Bauernfeind, Hazel Beeler, John Beeler, Kay Behrensmeyer, Miriam Belmaker, Mike Benjamin, Vadim Berg, Barry Berkovitz, Serena Bianchi, Felicitas Bidlack, Robert Blumenschine, René Bobe, Silvia Bortoluzzi, Frank Brown, Judith Brown, Peter Brown, Marta Camps, Sarah Cassel, Habiba Chirchir, Rich Cifelli, Robert Cifelli, Bryanne Colby, Paul Constantino, Raymond Corbey, Robin Crompton, Daryl de Ruiter, Alexandra de Sousa, Eric Delson, Catherine Denial, Alnawaz Devani, Marietta Dindo, Michelle Drapeau, Logan Ferree, Reid Ferring, John Fleagle, Jens Franzen, Kate Freeman, Pascal Gagneux, Emmanuel Gilissen, John Gowlett, Jay Greene, Nicole Griffin, Ana Gracia, Eileen Le Guillou, Frank Guy, Brian Hall, Will Harcourt-Smith, Malcolm Harman, the late Elizabeth Harmon, Geoffrey Harrison, Terry Harrison, William Hart, Claire Heckel, Simon Hillson, Leslea Hlusko, Louise Humphreys, David Hunt, Kevin Hunt, Joel Irish, Adam Jagich, Kayla Jarvis, William Jungers, Jon Kalb, John Kappelman, Richard Klein, Chris Kocher, Steve Kuhn, Joel Kuipers, Ottmar Kulmer, Joanna Lambert, Beth Lawrie, Daniel Lieberman, Julien Louys, Shannon McFarlin, Andrew McGurn, Lindsay McHenry, Jeff McKee, Roberto Machiarelli, Daniel Miller, Ignacio Martínez, Mary Marzke, Jim Moore, Michael Morwood, Jackson Njau, Rick Potts, Bryan Pratt, Yoel Rak, Jean-Paul Raynal, Safia Razzuqi, Kaye Reed, Liz Renner, Vernon Reynolds, Philip Rightmire, Mirjana Roksandic, Lorenzo Rook, Chris Ruff, Margaret Schoeninger, Friedemann Schrenk, Geralyn Schulz, Sileshi Semaw, Brian Shea, John Shea, Courtney Snell, Katya Stansfield, Christine Steininger, Chikako Suda-King, Fred Szalay, Mark Teaford, Matt Tocheri, Peter Ungar, Sidney Vasquez, Julian Waller, Steve Ward, Randy White, Tim White, David Wilkinson, Phillip Williams, Pamela Willoughby, Milford Wolpoff, Barth Wright, Roshna Wunderlich, and Bernhard Zipfel. My thanks also to Francisco Ayala for his interest in the project and for finding the time to write his fine Foreword. I have come to enjoy Francisco’s friendship through CARTA, the brainchild of Ajit Varki, and now co-directed by Fred Gage, Margaret Schoeninger, and Ajit Varki; its meetings and its members have done much to widen my intellectual horizons. All of us involved in CARTA are grateful to the G. Harold and Leila Y. Mathers Foundation and its Executive Director, James Handelman, for their long-term support for CARTA and to Pascal Gagneux, the Associate Director, and Linda Nelson for organizing it and its meetings. Those of you who know me will realize that I am the last person who should have been entrusted with the task of making sure that thousands of entries were curated so that only the latest versions find their way into the final text; my skills as a curator of references are also sorely lacking. It is no exaggeration to say that without the help of a series of student colleagues, especially Jennifer Baker, Kaitlyn Baraldi, Alex Claxton, Gabriel Mallozzi, and Erin Mikels, that this project would still be a work in progress; Kayla Jarvis also helped in many ways. Alex Claxton set up the topic entry list and collated the master reference file; my thanks to them both. Sometimes serendipity comes to one’s aid, and in my case it came in the form of Amanda Henry. She just happened to have defended her thesis at the time of greatest need for the W-BEHE and she was willing to pitch in and help bring the project to fruition. Without her hard work, determination, stamina, common sense, and intelligence we would still be assembling entries. Working with her has been my pleasure and privilege; it was also fun. Amanda and Jennifer Baker read and commented on every entry before they were sent for copy-editing.

    My notes remind me that I met with Jane Huber (then an Editor at Blackwell Publishing) in Boston at the end of June 2005. Jane’s encouragement and efficiency were the reason I decided to work with Blackwell on this project, and although she left to work for Oxfam it proved a wise decision for as many will realize Blackwell subsequently merged with Wiley, a publisher with deep connections with paleoanthropology via the American Journal of Physical Anthropology and Evolutionary Anthropology. Andy Slade has been my supervising editor and W-BEHE’s champion and I thank him for his efforts on its behalf. I was fortunate that Kelvin Matthews, Project Editor for Life Sciences, has been the lead person in the editing and production process within Wiley-Blackwell. His quiet but insistent determination to make sure that W-BEHE happened and his behind-the-scenes work to keep the project on track are sincerely appreciated. Among the many reasons I am grateful to Kelvin is that it was he who arranged for Nik Prowse to be the copy-editor. Nik works from Durham, England, and he and I did not meet until after W-BEHE was published until after, but it is no exaggeration to say that without his intelligence, attention to detail (he could have been a neurosurgeon), knowledge, common sense, and good humor the project might well have foundered. The world is replete with people who create problems; Nik (and Kelvin) belong to a much smaller number who were put into this world to solve them.

    My sincere thanks are also due to my colleagues at George Washington University. They have tolerated my distraction with W-BEHE and several have made

    Enjoying the preview?
    Page 1 of 1